Featured Topics

Featured series.

A series of random questions answered by Harvard experts.

Explore the Gazette

Read the latest.

Heidi Schreck in Farkas Hall.

A Chekhov play relatable to Americans today

Grisha Coleman teaching the Feldenkrais Method.

Gain without pain

Two models pose during the fashion show.

Everything, everywhere, all at once (kind of)

Images courtesy of John Patton, Ed Hagen, Samuel Mehr, Manvir Singh, and Luke Glowacki

Music everywhere

Comprehensive study explains that it is universal and that some songs sound ‘right’ in different social contexts, all over the world

Jed Gottlieb

Harvard Correspondent

Henry Wadsworth Longfellow wrote, “Music is the universal language of mankind.” Scientists at Harvard have just published the most comprehensive scientific study to date on music as a cultural product, which supports the American poet’s pronouncement and examines what features of song tend to be shared across societies.

The study was conceived by Samuel Mehr, a fellow of the Harvard Data Science Initiative and research associate in psychology, Manvir Singh, a graduate student in the Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, and Luke Glowacki, formerly a Harvard graduate student and now a professor of anthropology at Pennsylvania State University.

They set out to address big questions: Is music a cultural universal? If that’s a given, which musical qualities overlap across disparate societies? If it isn’t, why does it seem so ubiquitous? But they needed a data set of unprecedented breadth and depth. Over a five-year period, the team hunted down hundreds of recordings in libraries and private collections of scientists half a world away.

“We are so used to being able to find any piece of music that we like on the internet,” said Mehr, who is now a principal investigator at Harvard’s Music Lab . “But there are thousands and thousands of recordings buried in archives. At one point, we were looking for traditional Celtic music and we found a call number in the [Harvard] library system and librarian told us we needed to wait on the other side of the library because there was more room over there. Twenty minutes later this poor librarian comes out with a cart of about 20 cases of reel-to-reel recordings of Celtic music.”

Mehr added those reel tapes to the team’s growing discography, combining it with a corpus of ethnography containing nearly 5,000 descriptions of songs from 60 human societies. Mehr, Singh, and Glowacki call this database The Natural History of Song .

Their questions were so compelling that the project rapidly grew into a major international collaboration with musicians, data scientists, psychologists, linguists, and political scientists. Published in Science this week, it represents the team’s most ambitious study yet about music.

Manvir Singh, a graduate student in Harvard’s department of Human Evolutionary Biology, studied indigenous music and performance as a part of his fieldwork. Here Mentawai children in Siberut Island, Indonesia, are practicing in a kitchen. Video courtesy of Manvir Singh.

Music appears in every society observed.

“As a graduate student, I was working on studies of infant music perception, and I started to see all these studies that made claims about music being universal,” Mehr said. “How is it that every paper on music starts out with this big claim, but there’s never a citation backing that up … Now we can back that up.”

They looked at every society for which there was ethnographic information in a large online database, 315 in all, and found mention of music in all of them. For the discography, they collected 118 songs from a total of 86 cultures, covering 30 geographic regions. And they added the ethnographic material they’d collected.

“I started to see all these studies that made claims about music being universal. How is it that every paper on music starts out with this big claim but there’s never a citation backing that up … Now we can back that up.” Samuel Mehr, researcher

The team and their researchers coded the ethnography and discography that makes up the Natural History of Song into dozens of variables. They logged details about singers and audience members, the time of day and duration of singing, the presence of instruments, and more for thousands of passages about songs in the ethnographic corpus. The discography was analyzed four different ways: machine summaries, listener ratings, expert annotations, expert transcriptions.

They found that, across societies, music is associated with behaviors such as infant care, healing, dance, and love (among many others, like mourning, warfare, processions, and ritual). Examining lullabies, healing songs, dance songs, and love songs in particular, they discovered that songs that share behavioral functions tend to have similar musical features.

“Lullabies and dance songs are ubiquitous, and they are also highly stereotyped,” Singh said. “For me, dance songs and lullabies tend to define the space of what music can be. They do very different things with features that are almost the opposite of each other.”

The unanswered questions of music, according to researcher Manvir Singh

Transcript:.

So in this project we asked, “What is universal about music, and what varies?” This is a deep question in the study of humanity. Music is this widespread behavior but until now, we actually have not known, there have been a lot of unanswered questions about what these patterns are. There’s something that I find appealing about this array of humans doing all of these different things, producing music — which is this beautiful cultural product, but there being these echoes of similarity, or this echo of structure? That’s what I find appealing about the question and the project. But there’s also the more academic side, which is: Music is this ubiquitous human behavior. And it’s something that people engage in daily in societies around the world. And yet we understand so little about it. Obviously, music is hugely diverse, even within a classroom of students — the kind of music they listen to, engage with, and produce is, like, hugely different. But I think there’s something comforting and colorful about the fact that in this huge web of diversity there is something that we share, that we’re all speaking to.”

Definitely seeing music as cross-cultural excites Singh because he comes to the Natural History of Song project as someone who studies the social, cognitive, and cultural evolutionary foundations of complex traditions found throughout societies from music to law, narrative to witchcraft.

For Mehr, who began his academic life in music education, the study looks toward unlocking the governing rules of “musical grammar.” That idea has been percolating among music theorists, linguists, and psychologists of music for decades, but has never been demonstrated across cultures.

“In music theory, tonality is often assumed to be an invention of Western music, but our data raise the controversial possibility that this could be a universal feature of music,” he said. “That raises pressing questions about structure that underlies music everywhere — and whether and how our minds are designed to make music.”

This study was supported in part by the Harvard Data Science Initiative, an NIH Director’s Early Independence Award, the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program, and the Microsoft Research postdoctoral fellowship program.

Share this article

More like this.

music has no boundaries essay

Songs in the key of humanity

music has no boundaries essay

The look of music

Yvette Jackson

New faculty: Yvette J. Jackson

You might like.

At first, Heidi Schreck wasn’t sure the world needed another take on ‘Uncle Vanya’

Grisha Coleman teaching the Feldenkrais Method.

OFA dance classes offer well-being through movement

Two models pose during the fashion show.

There’s never a shortage of creativity on campus. But during Arts First, it all comes out to play.

Epic science inside a cubic millimeter of brain

Researchers publish largest-ever dataset of neural connections

How far has COVID set back students?

An economist, a policy expert, and a teacher explain why learning losses are worse than many parents realize

Excited about new diet drug? This procedure seems better choice.

Study finds minimally invasive treatment more cost-effective over time, brings greater weight loss

David Ludden Ph.D.

Is Music a Universal Language?

Expressing the shared human experience..

Posted July 31, 2015 | Reviewed by Ekua Hagan

Music is a universal language. Or so musicians like to claim. “With music,” they’ll say, “you can communicate across cultural and linguistic boundaries in ways that you can’t with ordinary languages like English or French.”

On one level, this statement is obviously true. You don’t have to speak French to enjoy a composition by Debussy. But is music really a universal language? That depends on what you mean by “universal” and what you mean by “language.”

Every human culture has music, just as each has language. So it’s true that music is a universal feature of the human experience. At the same time, both music and linguistic systems vary widely from culture to culture. In fact, unfamiliar musical systems may not even sound like music. I’ve overheard Western-trained music scholars dismiss Javanese gamelan as “clanging pots” and traditional Chinese opera as “cackling hens.”

Nevertheless, studies show that people are pretty good at detecting the emotions conveyed in unfamiliar music idioms—that is, at least the two basic emotions of happiness and sadness. Specific features of melody contribute to the expression of emotion in music. Higher pitch, more fluctuations in pitch and rhythm, and faster tempo convey happiness, while the opposite conveys sadness.

Perhaps then we have an innate musical sense. But language also has melody—which linguists call prosody. Exactly these same features—pitch, rhythm, and tempo—are used to convey emotion in speech, in a way that appears to be universal across languages.

Listen in on a conversation in French or Japanese or some other language you don’t speak. You won’t understand the content, but you will understand the shifting emotional states of the speakers. She’s upset, and he’s getting defensive. Now she’s really angry, and he’s backing off. He pleads with her, but she doesn’t buy it. He starts sweet-talking her, and she resists at first but slowly gives in. Now they’re apologizing and making up...

We understand this exchange in a foreign language because we know what it sounds like in our own language. Likewise, when we listen to a piece of music, either from our culture or from another, we infer emotion on the basis of melodic cues that mimic universal prosodic cues. In this sense, music truly is a universal system for communicating emotion.

But is music a kind of language? Again, we have to define our terms. In everyday life, we often use “language” to mean “communication system.” Biologists talk about the “language of bees,” which is a way to tell hive mates about the location of a new source of nectar.

Florists talk about the “language of flowers,” through which their customers can express their relationship intentions. “Red roses mean…. Pink carnations mean… Yellow daffodils mean…” (I’m not a florist, so I don’t speak flower.)

And then there’s “ body language .” By this we mean the postures, gestures, movements and facial expressions we use to convey emotions, social status, and so on. Although we often use body language when we speak, linguists don’t consider it a true form of language. Instead, it’s a communication system, just as are the so-called languages of bees and flowers.

By definition, language is a communication system consisting of (1) a set of meaningful symbols (words) and (2) a set of rules for combining those symbols (syntax) into larger meaningful units (sentences). While many species have communication systems, none of these count as a language because they lack one or the other component.

The alarm and food calls of many species consist of a set of meaningful symbols, but they lack rules for combining those symbols. Likewise, bird song and whale song have rules for combining elements, but these elements aren’t meaningful symbols. Only the song as a whole has meaning—“Hey ladies, I’m hot,” and “Hey other guys, stay away!”

music has no boundaries essay

Like language, music has syntax—rules for ordering elements—such as notes, chords, and intervals—into complex structures. Yet none of these elements has meaning on its own. Rather, it’s the larger structure—the melody—that conveys emotional meaning. And it does that by mimicking the prosody of speech.

Since music and language share features in common, it’s not surprising that many of the brain areas that process language also process music. But this doesn’t mean that music is language. Part of the misunderstanding comes from the way we tend to think about specific areas of the brain as having specific functions. Any complex behavior, whether language or music or driving a car, will recruit contributions from many different brain areas.

Music certainly isn’t a universal language in the sense that you could use it to express any thought to any person on the planet. But music does have the power to evoke deep primal feelings at the core of the shared human experience. It not only crosses cultures, it also reaches deep into our evolutionary past. And it that sense, music truly is a universal language.

David Ludden is the author of The Psychology of Language: An Integrated Approach .

Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Slevc, L. R., Okada, B. M. (2015). Processing structure in language and music: a case for shared reliance on cognitive control. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 637-652.

Tan, S.-L., Pfordresher, P., & Harré, R. (2010). Psychology of music: from sound to significance . New York, NY: Psychology Press.

David Ludden Ph.D.

David Ludden, Ph.D. , is a professor of psychology at Georgia Gwinnett College.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Therapy Center NEW
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

March 2024 magazine cover

Understanding what emotional intelligence looks like and the steps needed to improve it could light a path to a more emotionally adept world.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

Roosevelt Review

The Power of Music to Help Change the World (and Me!)

by Paul Wertico, CCPA Associate Professor of Jazz

Wow — it’s sometimes hard to fathom that the year 1968 was half a century ago and at a time when, comparatively speaking, life seemed relatively simple to life as we know it today.

music has no boundaries essay

As an avid listener, I’d routinely hear songs on the radio that I liked (either by an artist I knew, or one who was new), and I’d eventually sing along with those songs pretty much whenever I heard them. The lyrics were sometimes audible and distinguishable, and other times only some of them were, in which case I made up the rest of the “lyrics” with whatever words or syllables I felt like singing. Although a song’s sound, groove, harmony and melody were also important to me, singing along with the song was often so much fun that sometimes the “fun part” actually masked the song’s intended meaning; or in certain cases, my life’s experiences and worldly knowledge hadn’t evolved yet to the point where I was able to truly decipher the actual meaning and message of the songs.

When I reflect now on what my life was like as a 15-year-old back in 1968, it makes me realize how much of those songs I actually “heard” and how much I actually “missed.”

Growing up as a white teenager living in the predominantly white suburb of Cary, Illinois, my life was quite different from the various situations and living conditions described in the lyrics of certain songs I listened to. Even though the radio programmers were no doubt primarily interested in whatever songs would appeal to their station’s listeners (and therefore bring in money and advertisers to those stations), knowingly or unknowingly, they also performed a service to the community and to young people like myself by spreading the word and enlightening listeners through certain songs’ insightful and even profound lyrics that portrayed the outside world as a much bigger — and in many cases “badder” — place than my bedroom, my parent’s car, or wherever a radio was playing.

“When I reflect now on what my life was like as a 15-year-old back in 1968, it makes me realize how much of those songs I actually ‘heard’ and how much I actually ‘missed.’”

– Paul Wertico CCPA Associate Professor of Jazz

music has no boundaries essay

More in this section

Roosevelt receives $5 million grant to expand stem doctoral programs and research facilities

Roosevelt receives $5 million grant to expand stem doctoral programs and research facilities

Awarded by the U.S. Department of Education and given to only nine institutions nationwide, the grant is part of a national effort to assist historically underserved student populations pursue a career in STEM fields and improve graduation rates.

  • Learn to fundraise for Save The Music
  • Start a Fundraiser on Facebook
  • Start a Fundraiser on Classy
  • Pledge your birthday

Home › Blog › Advocacy › How Does Music Affect Society?

November 3, 2021

How Does Music Affect Society?

By Lia Peralta

How Does Music Affect Society?

Music has shaped cultures and societies around the world for generations. It has the power to alter one’s mood, change perceptions, and inspire change. While everyone has a personal relationship with music, its effects on the culture around us may not be immediately apparent. So, how does music affect society ? The impact of music on society is broad and deeply ingrained in our history. To demonstrate how deeply our lives are affected by music, let’s delve into the sociological effects of music and how it affects culture.

How Does Music Affect Society

Music is an essential aspect of all human civilizations and has the power to emotionally, morally, and culturally affect society. When people from one culture exchange music with each other, they gain valuable insight into another way of life. Learning how music and social bonding are linked is especially crucial in times of conflict when other lines of communication prove to be challenging.

Music, as a cultural right, may aid in the promotion and protection of other human rights. It can help in the healing process, dismantling walls and boundaries, reconciliation, and education. Around the world, music is being used as a vehicle for social change and bringing communities together.

At the core of our everyday experience with music, we use it to relax, express ourselves, come to terms with our emotions, and generally improve our well-being. It has evolved into a tool for healing and self-expression, often dictating how we, as individuals, take steps to impact society.

Why is Music Beneficial to Society?

Music has the power to connect with and influence people in a way that feels fundamentally different from other forms of communication. Humans often feel that “no one understands them” or knows how they “truly feel.” Many resort to music to find connections with others to express themselves or find a sense of understanding among peers.

How does music affect our lives? Music has the ability to deeply affect our mental states and raise our mood. When we need it, music gives us energy and motivation. When we’re worried, it can soothe us; when we’re weary, it can encourage us; and when we’re feeling deflated, it can re-inspire us. It even functions to improve our physical health, as it’s been proven that high-tempo music results in better workouts . We connect with others via music, especially those who produce or perform it — we recite their lyrics, dance to their melodies, and form a sense of connection through their self-expression.

Songs and melodies have the power to inspire people, guide their actions, and aid in the formation of identities. Music can unite people – even if absorbed in solitude, capture your imagination and boost creativity. A person who has been affected by music is not alone. They are among the masses trying to find their role in society and form connections with others. Thankfully, while it can help us “find ourselves,” music influence on society can also be seen in:

– Providing a platform for the underrepresented to speak out

– Affecting mood and inspiration

– Helping us cope by encouraging us to express ourselves through movement and dancing

– Bridging a divide in communication

– Creating a venue for education and idea-sharing

Music’s Effect on Our Thoughts and Actions

Music’s effect on the self is far-reaching, tapping into our memories, subconscious thoughts, emotions, and interests. Thanks to the music artists who have put their heart and soul into creating, we feel connected with other people and their difficulties, challenges, and emotions. So much about our brains is still being discovered but through neurology, we are learning more and more about how music affects us.

We all know that being exposed to music’s beauty, rhythm, and harmony significantly influences how we feel. We also know that music emotionally impacts us, reaching into forgotten memories and connecting us to ourselves. Music therapy is often used to improve attention and memory, and can have a positive effect on those suffering from dementia or Alzheimers. Music has the potential to be a powerful healing tool in a variety of ways and pervades every aspect of our existence. Songs are used to define spiritual ceremonies, toddlers learn the alphabet via rhyme and verse, and malls and restaurants, where we choose to spend our free time, are rarely silent.

But how much can this ever-present object influence our behavior and emotions? According to research , music has a significant impact on humans. It can potentially affect disease, depression, expenditure, productivity, and our outlook on life. The impact of music on our brain is being better understood thanks to advances in neuroscience and the examination of music’s impact on the brain. It has been shown via brain scans that when we listen to or perform music, nearly all brain regions are active simultaneously. Listening to and making music may actually changes the way your brain works .

According to studies, music impacts how we view the world around us. In a 2011 research paper , 43 students were given the job of recognizing happy and sad faces while listening to happy or sad music in the background by researchers from the University of Groningen. Participants noticed more cheerful faces when upbeat music was played, while the opposite was true when sad music was played. According to the researchers, music’s effect might be because our perceptual decisions on sensory stimuli, such as facial expressions in this experiment, are directly impacted by our mental state. Music triggers physical responses in the brain and puts in motion a series of chemical reactions.

In the book, Classical Music: Expected the Unexpected , author and conductor Kent Nagano spoke with neuroscientist Daniel J. Levitin on how music interacts with the brain. The sociological effects of music can include the improvement of people’s well-being due to chemical reactions in the brain, such as an increase in oxytocin. Oxytocin, or the “love hormone,” makes us more inclined to engage in social interactions or build trust between individuals. Music also boosts the synthesis of the immunoglobulin A antibody, which is crucial for human health. Studies have also shown that melatonin, adrenaline, and noradrenaline levels increase after only a few weeks of music therapy. The hormones noradrenaline and adrenaline cause us to become more alert, experience excitement, and cause the brain’s “reward” regions to fire.

The Cultural Impact of Music

Today’s popular music reflects the culture of the day. But, how does music affect society over time? How has music changed over the past century? In the lyrics and sound of each era, we can discern the imprints of a particular generation and see history in the making. And, in this day and age, culture is changing faster than ever before, mirroring musical forms that are evolving and emerging at the same rate.

For decades, the effects of music on society have been a source of contention, and it seems that with each generation, a new musical trend emerges that has the previous generations saying, “Well back in my day, we had…”. Music and social movements are intrinsically linked together. Almost every popular kind of music was considered scandalous back in the day, and the dancing that accompanied jazz, rock ‘n’ roll, and hip-hop drew protests and boycotts from all around. Just look at The Beatles, who were considered scandalous by the older generation when they first arrived on the music scene.

While we may not like a new music trend or a particular genre of music, we must also take a step back and appreciate how lucky we are to be exposed to it at all. Music in some parts of the world is not as easily accessible. While music has always been a means of pushing the boundaries of expression, it’s clear that the world isn’t expressing itself in the same way. The various musical trends we’ve seen in just this lifetime provide an insightful look into what is and isn’t being discussed in some cultures.

Music as an Agent for Change

Another essential factor to consider is how strongly music influences society and, thus, human behavior. Music’s impact on human rights movements and its role as an agent for change is clear in the history books. One example is the impact of the “freedom songs” of the Civil Rights movement, such as “We Shall Overcome” and “Strange Fruit.” These songs broke down barriers, educated people, built empathy across the divide, and had a hand in ending segregation. Music today continues to shed light on the inequalities experienced by people worldwide, and it’s clear that music will never stop acting as an agent for change.

Because of how strong of an influence melodies and lyrics have on society, we must be acutely aware of our current culture. Still, more importantly, we must be conscious of the cultures we wish to build and develop via our music. Songs have the power to change the world in unexpected ways, challenging preconceived notions and shedding light on issues that have historically been ignored.

Music’s Impact on Youth

How does having music education impact youth? We know from our experience that music in schools improves student, teacher and community outcomes – and in turn, society, specifically the future generation. In a case study about our work in Newark, NJ , 68% of teachers reported improved academic performance. 94% of teachers also saw improvement in social-emotional skills. Schools saw better attendance and ELA (English Language Arts) scores.

Another example of this is our work in Metro Nashville Public Schools , which has been a partner district of Save The Music since 1999. Students who participated in music programs for up to one year had significantly better attendance and graduation rates, higher GPAs and test scores, and lower discipline reports than their non-music peers. Students with more than one year of music participation performed significantly better than their peers with less on each of these indicators.

We Invest in Music in Schools

Whether you’re a music buff or not, anyone can appreciate the impact music has on society. A great way to show this appreciation is by being part of music’s impact on the world and learning how you can help facilitate change. Save The Music is a music foundation that collaborates with public school districts to provide grants for music education instructors and school administrators in the form of new instruments, technology , and online music education . Our initiative helps schools get their music programs off the ground and keep students inspired.

Contact us today to learn how you can save the music through our music education advocacy programs .

Advocacy , Blog

Subscribe to our newsletter

Interested in working with us contact, how music can transcend language and impact the world, march 7, 2022, written by:, marina azcárate.

Time and time again we have heard that music is a universal language. But what exactly does that mean?

This means that music has the unique power to unite people across all boards globally. Regardless of race, class, gender identity and/or expression, religion, age, you name it, music can bring us together. If we take a minute to think that two human beings will communicate mostly via one of countless languages and dialects and that when two people don’t speak the same language communication becomes near impossible for some individuals, the power of music becomes even more astonishing. Melodies take the place of words.

While it is true that oftentimes, the most popular music in each country is that in its own language, there have been many artists who have broken the barriers of language with their music. Nowadays, this debate has become increasingly relevant as we begin to see non-English songs dominate the international charts, notably Latin music and K-Pop.

music has no boundaries essay

A song can serve the purpose of replacing a whole conversation, through which two or more people can tune into, entering the same wavelength by the end of it and having reached a mutual understanding of one another. Who knows, maybe the world would be a better place if language disappeared and we had to communicate via music.

In this sense, musicians kind of serve as unofficial ambassadors of intercultural communication. Cross-culture shamans.

But why is this important? Why does it matter if music can transcend language or not?

Well, it matters because if used correctly music can be one of the most transformative tools at hand that we have. It can affect social change, and prod society to evolve, helping us walk hand in hand towards a better tomorrow.

Music has been breaking language barriers for quite some time now

An early historical example of music breaking the boundaries of music would be opera. Opera famously began in Italy in the 1500s, and it eventually spread out all across the globe. An art based on storytelling, that somehow managed to wonderfully depict the most epic of tales in a foreign language. And let us tell ya, there weren’t any subtitles back then.

Now of course as opera evolved in each nation, many national composers began writing operas in their own language, but still, the favoured language was often Italian or Latin. With German and French following suit. In fact, opera is possibly one of the only music genres that haven’t been so thoroughly invaded by the English language.

While we might dismiss opera as not being all that impactful for music today, we need to remember that before opera, in Europe, grandiose music was mostly listened to in churches, cathedrals, or places of the sort. As such, music during the renaissance was sternly tied to religion and religious stories. With opera, music began its journey towards secularism once again.

French music saved lives during WW2, literally.

Possibly one of the greatest examples of music breaking down language barriers, and being subsequently used for the greater good is the case of French iconic singer, Edith Piaf, during World War 2. The singer was invited to sing in Germany, and much to the dismay of her nation she accepted. Under two conditions: Firstly she stated that if she was to sing, she would do so for everyone, meaning both German troops and French prisoner Jews. Secondly, she wanted to take photos with everyone after each performance.

music has no boundaries essay

Her music was so revered, that the Germans accepted. Following her concerts, Edith Piaf would pass the photos on to her influential friends who would then create false documents for the Jews in the photos. Finally, the French singer would return to Germany for another tour and once again after the concerts during the signings and photos, she would secretly slip the documents to the prisoners, helping free thousands of Jews during the war.

Now, of course, Edith Piaf was an exceptional case. But lives were literally saved because her music was so powerful, that even the Germans wanted to bask in the sheer marvellousness of her voice. Regardless of whether she was singing in French. And these are the Nazis we’re talking about, who, you know, kind of was a “their-language-above-all-else-kinda-people”.

Still, while incredibly heroic and impactful examples exist of the boundary-breaking, positive impact of music, it’s not all a bed of roses.

But music isn’t always enough, and it doesn’t always work

Truth is, although of course music can transcend language, and be an incredible tool for social and political change, it doesn’t always work.

Bob Marley transcended language, time, and cultures. But he couldn’t do it all.

Sometimes music simply isn’t enough, even when speaking the same language. Bob Marley is an incredibly iconic cultural figure, there is no denying that. He lives on in the collective memory and will most probably continue to do so for centuries. Most people know his face, his voice, his music. His impact has been such that essentially all reggae music is associated with him. Chances are you’ll see the colours red, yellow, and green, and one of Bob Marley’s songs will subconsciously begin playing in your mind. Generation after generation, Bob Marley’s songs are incorporated into contemporary culture as hymns and odes to togetherness, to unity, to universal love and solidarity.

Bob Marley’s music has undoubtedly transcended the barriers not only of language but of time too. But there was one thing he couldn’t do, help his country when it was most in need.

It was 1978, and Jamaica was on the verge of civil war between both major political parties, the Peoples National Party (PNP), and the Jamaican Labour Party (JLP). Both parties hired, essentially, gangsters for protection. Yet the tension was such, that even the local gang members thought the violence had to come to an end. And so they (the actual gang members) came up with the idea of having a concert for peace. In fact, it was one of the gang members, Claudius “Claudie” Massop who travelled to London to convince a then exiled Bob Marley to participate.

music has no boundaries essay

During his performance at the concert, Marley famously brought up the two opposing political leaders Michael Manley and Edward Seaga took their hands and held them up together. That moment will forever be revered in history, and for a bit there, one could even believe that the violence was over. Unfortunately, two years after the concert the event organizers were killed, and the next election year saw double the reported murders.

Music breaking language barriers today

One can’t dispute the fact that over the past few decades. English music has dominated the international charts, with the occasional foreign-language song miraculously “making it through”. It’s quite a common thing for a country to listen to their own national artist, and then the typical English speaking ones. The whole world knows Beyoncé, Lady Gaga, The Beatles, Kanye West, Michael Jackson or Eminem.

This is due in part, to the global hegemony held by the US during the last fifty years or more. But guess what? Now the US seems to be losing its position culturally, politically, economically, and socially as the world leader, and other cultures are breaking through and seeping into the global consciousness.

music has no boundaries essay

Today, international charts are seeing a huge shift in the languages present in the most listened to lists. One look at  Youtube’s most listened to songs of 2021  will confirm this. Why Youtube you ask? Well, its streaming services reach is about 1.9 billion people per month, surpassing by far any other service. Spanish and Indian language songs are in fact the most listened to, followed closely by Korean and English.

K-Pop completely obliterates language barriers in music

It’s true that Indian songs are possibly listened to mostly by the Eastern side of the globe, but there is no doubt that Latin music and K-pop has completely obliterated the English music hegemony. These two styles have taken over the globe, and are the pure example of music transcending language barriers.

The K-Pop idols are known for being a source of inspiration and empowerment to the youth. Their distinct aesthetics and vibrant music have truly had an impact when it comes to youth culture and expressing one’s individuality.

music has no boundaries essay

Latin music belongs everywhere, and so do Latin people

Similarly, the rise in Latin music has highlighted the cultural relevance and importance of Latin populations in several countries. Latino communities around the globe have seen their culture appreciated, respected, and for the first time regarded as being on the same level as any other English-speaking artist. This development has marked a historical moment where Latin music isn’t just for Latin people, it’s for everyone. In the same way that Latin people don’t just belong in Latin countries, they belong everywhere.

This issue is particularly eye-opening in countries such as the US, where the Latin community has been discriminated against and oppressed for decades, and continues to live and fight systemic racism.

Thanks to music, things that before were previously frowned upon when you had foreign heritage, such as speaking a different language, are now praised and appreciated. Diversity is increasingly being celebrated and treated as something that enriches society rather than threatens it. And it is partly due to the contributions of music. Let’s not forget it, and let’s continue to push forward together.

Trust us when we say, the soundtrack of your life will be so much better if you listen beyond the words.

  • Lifestyle Music

music has no boundaries essay

Masters of the Creative Craft, Creative Directors Edition

music has no boundaries essay

New Realities Of Love: Is Ethical Non-Monogamy For Real?

music has no boundaries essay

Giving Credit Where Credit’s Due, Producers Edition

Subscribe to stay informed of everything in our world.

music has no boundaries essay

Don't miss any news

Privacy Overview

music has no boundaries essay

Meller – Halloween Campaign

music has no boundaries essay

Gisela – 249 The Secret

music has no boundaries essay

IGWT – Create Your Balance

music has no boundaries essay

Born Living Yoga – Fall Campaign

The Relationship Between Music, Culture, and Society: Meaning in Music

  • First Online: 14 August 2018

Cite this chapter

music has no boundaries essay

  • Georgina Barton 2  

2211 Accesses

7 Citations

Music is inextricably linked with the context in which it is produced, consumed and taught and the inter-relationship between music, society and culture has been researched for many decades. Seminal research in the field of ethnomusicology has explored how social and cultural customs influence music practices in macro and micro ways. Music, for example, can be a core feature of social celebrations such as weddings to a means of cultural expression and maintenance through ceremony. It is this meaning that can alter across contexts and therefore reflected in the ways music and sound is manipulated or constructed to form larger works with different purposes. Whether to entertain or play a crucial role in ceremonial rituals music practices cannot be separated from the environment in which it exists.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Almén, B. (2017). A theory of musical narrative . Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

Book   Google Scholar  

Andersen, W. M., & Lawrence, J. E. (1991). Integrating music into the classroom (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.

Google Scholar  

Baldwin, J. R., Faulkner, S. L., Hecht, M. L., & Lindsley, S. L. (2008). Redefining culture: Perspectives across the disciplines . London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Barton, G. M. (2004). The influence of culture on instrumental music teaching: A participant-observation case study of Karnatic and Queensland instrumental music teachers in context . Unpublished PhD thesis, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia.

Barton, G. M. (2006). The real state of music education: What students and teachers think. In E. MacKinlay (Ed.), Music Education, Research and Innovation journal (MERI) . Sydney, Australia: Australian Society for Music Education.

Blacking, J. (1973). How musical is man? Seattle and London: University of Washington Press.

Born, G., & Hesmondhalgh, D. (2000). Western music and its others: Difference, representation and appropriation in music . Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.

Boyce-Tillman, J. (1996). A framework for intercultural dialogue. In M. Floyd (Ed.), World musics in education (pp. 43–49). Hants, UK: Scholar Press.

Brennan, P. S. (1992). Design and implementation of curricula experiences in world music: A perspective. In H. Lees (Ed.), Music education: Sharing musics of the world (pp. 221–225). Seoul, Korea: Conference Proceedings from World Conference International Society of Music Education.

Campbell, P. S. (1991). Lessons from the world: A cross-cultural guide to music teaching and learning . New York: Schirmer Books.

Campbell, P. S. (Ed.). (1996). Music in cultural context: Eight views on world music education . Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference.

Campbell, P. S. (2004). Teaching music globally: Experiencing music, expressing culture . New York: Oxford University Press.

Campbell, P. S. (2016). World music pedagogy: Where music meets culture in classroom practice. In C. R. Abril & B. M. Gault (Eds.), Teaching general music: Approaches, issues and viewpoints (pp. 89–11). New York: Oxford University Press.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Dewey, J. (1958). Art as experience . New York: Capricorn Books.

Dillon, S. C. (2001). The student as maker: An examination of the meaning of music to students in a school and the ways in which we give access to meaningful music education . Unpublished PhD thesis, La Trobe University, Victoria, Australia.

Dunbar-Hall, P. (2009). Ethnopedagogy: Culturally contextualised learning and teaching as an agent of change. Action, Criticism, and Theory for Music Education, 8 (2), 60–78.

Dunbar-Hall, P., & Gibson, C. (2004). Deadly sounds, deadly places: Contemporary aboriginal music in Australia . Sydney: UNSW Press.

Dunbar-Hall, P., & Wemyss, K. (2000). The effects of the study of popular music on music education. International Journal of Music Education, 36 , 23–34.

Article   Google Scholar  

Elliott, D. J. (1994). Music, education and musical value. In H. Lees (Ed.), Musical connections: Tradition and change—Proceedings of the 21st World Conference of the International Society for Music Education (Vol. 21). Tampa, FL.

Elliott, D. J., & Silverman, M. (1995). Music matters: A new philosophy of music education . Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.

Ellis, C. J. (1985). Aboriginal music education for living: Cross-cultural experiences from South Australia . St Lucia, QLD: University of Queensland Press.

Feld, S. (1984). Sound structure as social structure. Journal of Ethnomusicology, 28 (3), 383–409.

Feld, S. (2013). Sound and sentiment: Birds, weeping, poetics and song in Kaluli expression . Durham and London: Duke University Press.

Gaunt, H., & Westerlund, H. (2013). Collaborative learning in higher music education . London: Routledge.

George, L. (1987). Teaching the music of six different cultures . Danbury, CT: World Music Press.

Glickman, T. (1996). Intercultural music education: The early years. Journal of the Indian Musicological Society, 27 , 52–58.

Gourlay, K. (1978). Towards a reassessment of the ethnomusicologist’s role in research. Journal of Ethnomusicology, 22 , 1–35.

Green, L. (1990). Music on deaf ears: Musical meaning, ideology, education . Manchester, UK: Manchester University Press.

Green, L. (2002). How popular musicians learn: A way ahead for music education . Aldershot, UK: Ashgate Publishing.

Green, L. (2011). Learning, teaching and musical identity: Voices across cultures . Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.

Hargreaves, D. J., Marshall, N. A., & North, A. C. (2003). Music education in the twenty-first century: A psychological perspective. British Journal of Music Education, 20 (2), 147–163.

Harwood, D. L. (1976). Universals in music: A perspective from cognitive psychology. Journal of Ethnomusicology, 20 , 521–533.

Herndon, M., & McLeod, N. (Eds.). (1982). Music as culture . Norwood, PA: Norwood Editions.

Ho, W.-C. (2014). Music education curriculum and social change: A study of popular music in secondary schools in Beijing, China. Music Education Research, 16 (3), 267–289.

Inskip, C., MacFarlane, A., & Rafferty, P. (2008). Meaning, communication, music: Towards a revised communication model. Journal of Documentation, 64 (5), 687–706. https://doi.org/10.1108/00220410810899718

Ishimatsu, N. (2014). Shifting phases of the art scene in Malaysia and Thailand: Comparing colonized and non-colonized countries. Engage! Public Intellectuals Transforming Society, 1 , 78–87.

Kelly, S. N. (2016). Teaching music in American society: A social and cultural understanding of music education (2nd ed.). New York and London: Routledge.

Lamasisi, F. (1992). Documentation of music and dance: What it means to the bearers of the traditions, their role and anxieties associated with the process. Oceania Monograph Australia, 41 , 222–234.

Langer, S. K. (1953). Feeling and form: A theory of art developed from philosophy in a new key . New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

Langer, S. K. (1957). Philosophy in a new key: A study of the symbolism of reason, rite and art . Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Leong, S. (2016). Glocalisation and interculturality in Chinese research: A planetary perspective. In P. Burnard, E. Mackinlay, & K. Powell (Eds.), Routledge handbook of intercultural arts research (pp. 344–357). London: Routledge.

Lomax, A. (1968). Folk song style and culture . Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science.

Lomax, A. (1976). Cantometrics: An approach to the anthropology of music . Berkeley: University of California Extension Media Centre.

Lundquist, B., & Szego, C. K. (Eds.). (1998). Musics of the world’s cultures: A source book for music educators . Nedlands, WA: CIRCME.

MacDonald, A., Barton, G. M., Baguley, M., & Hartwig, K. (2016). Teachers’ curriculum stories: Thematic perceptions and capacities. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 48 (13), 1336–1351. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131857.2016.1210496

McAllester, D. P. (1984). A problem in ethics. In J. C. Kassler & J. Stubington (Eds.), Problems and solutions: Occasional essays in musicology presented to Alice M. Moyle (pp. 279–289). Sydney, Australia: Hale and Iremonger.

McAllester, D. P. (1996). David P. McAllester on Navajo music. In P. S. Campbell (Ed.), Music in cultural context: Eight views on world music education (pp. 5–11). Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference.

Merriam, A. P. (1964). The anthropology of music . Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

Meyer, L. B. (1961). Emotion and meaning in music . Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Miller, T. E. (1996). Terry E. Miller on Thai music. In P. S. Campbell (Ed.), Music in cultural context: Eight views on world music education (pp. 5–11). Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference.

Nattiez, J. (1990). Music and discourse: Toward a semiology of music . Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Nettl, B. (1975). Music in primitive cultures: Iran, a recently developed nation. In C. Hamm, B. Nettl, & R. Byrnside (Eds.), Contemporary music and music cultures (pp. 71–100). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Nettl, B. (1992). Ethnomusicology and the teaching of world music. International Journal of Music Education, 20 , 3–7.

Oku, S. (1994). From the conceptual approach to the cross-arts approach. In S. Leong (Ed.), Music in schools and teacher education: A global perspective (pp. 115–132). Nedlands, WA: International Society for Music Education, Commission for Music in Schools and Teacher in Association with CIRCME.

Payne, H. (1988). Singing a sister’s sites: Women’s land rites in the Australian Musgrave ranges . Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD.

Pratt, G., Henson, M., & Cargill, S. (1998). Aural awareness: Principles and practice . Oxford: Oxford University Press on Demand.

Radocy, R., & Boyle, J. D. (1979). Psychological foundations of musical behaviour . Springfield, IL: C. C. Thomas.

Rampton, B. (2014). Crossing language and ethnicity among adolescents (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.

Ravignani, A., Delgado, T., & Kirby, S. (2016). Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals. Nature Human Behaviour, 1 (1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-016-0007

Reimer, B. (1989). A philosophy of music education (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Shepard, J., & Wicke, P. (1997). Music and cultural theory . Cambridge: Polity Press.

Small, C. (1996). Music, society, education . Hanover: University Press of New England.

Small, C. (1998). Musicking: The meanings of performing and listening . Hanover: University Press of New England.

Smith, R. G. (1998). Evaluating the initiation, application and appropriateness of a series of customised teaching and learning strategies designed to communicate musical and related understandings interculturally . Unpublished Doctor of Teaching thesis, Northern Territory University, Northern Territory.

Spearritt, G. D. (1980). The music of the Iatmul people of the middle Sepik river (Papua New Guinea): With special reference to instrumental music at Kandangai and Aibom . Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD.

Steier, F. (1991). Research and reflexivity . London: Sage Publications.

Stock, J. P. (1994). Concepts of world music and their integration within western secondary music education. International Journal of Music Education, 23 , 3–16.

Stone, R. M. (2016). Theory for ethnomusicology . London: Routledge.

Suzuki, S. (1982). Where love is deep: The writings of Shinichi Suzuki (K. Selden, Trans.). St. Louis, MO: Talent Education Journal.

Swanwick, K. (1996). A basis for music education (2nd ed.). Berks, UK: NFER Publishing Company.

Swanwick, K. (1999). Teaching music musically . London and New York: Routledge Publishers.

Swanwick, K. (2001). Musical development theories revisited. Music Education Research, 3 (2), 227–242. https://doi.org/10.1080/14613800120089278

Swanwick, K. (2016). A developing discourse in music education: The selected works of Keith Swanwick . London: Routledge Publishers.

Turek, R. (1996). The elements of music: Concepts and application . New York: McGraw-Hill.

Walker, R. (1990). Musical beliefs: Psychoacoustic, mythical and educational perspectives . New York and London: Teachers College Press Columbia University.

Walker, R. (1992). Open peer commentary: Musical knowledge: The saga of music in the national curriculum. Psychology of Music, 20 (2), 162–179.

Walker, R. (1998). Swanwick puts music education back in its western prison—A reply. International Journal of Music Education, 31 (1), 59–65.

Walker, R. (2001). The rise and fall of philosophies of music education: Looking backwards in order to see ahead. Research Studies in Music Education, 17 (1), 3–18.

Whitman, B. A. (2005). Learning the meaning of music . Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

School of Teacher Education and Early Childhood, University of Southern Queensland, Springfield Central, Brisbane, QLD, Australia

Georgina Barton

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Barton, G. (2018). The Relationship Between Music, Culture, and Society: Meaning in Music. In: Music Learning and Teaching in Culturally and Socially Diverse Contexts. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-95408-0_2

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-95408-0_2

Published : 14 August 2018

Publisher Name : Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-319-95407-3

Online ISBN : 978-3-319-95408-0

eBook Packages : Education Education (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

The human nature of music.

\r\nStephen Malloch,*

  • 1 Westmead Psychotherapy Program, Sydney Medical School, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 2 The MARCS Institute for Brain, Behaviour, and Development, Western Sydney University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 3 Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom

Music is at the centre of what it means to be human – it is the sounds of human bodies and minds moving in creative, story-making ways. We argue that music comes from the way in which knowing bodies (Merleau-Ponty) prospectively explore the environment using habitual ‘patterns of action,’ which we have identified as our innate ‘communicative musicality.’ To support our argument, we present short case studies of infant interactions using micro analyses of video and audio recordings to show the timings and shapes of intersubjective vocalizations and body movements of adult and child while they improvise shared narratives of meaning. Following a survey of the history of discoveries of infant abilities, we propose that the gestural narrative structures of voice and body seen as infants communicate with loving caregivers are the building blocks of what become particular cultural instances of the art of music, and of dance, theatre and other temporal arts. Children enter into a musical culture where their innate communicative musicality can be encouraged and strengthened through sensitive, respectful, playful, culturally informed teaching in companionship. The central importance of our abilities for music as part of what sustains our well-being is supported by evidence that communicative musicality strengthens emotions of social resilience to aid recovery from mental stress and illness. Drawing on the experience of the first author as a counsellor, we argue that the strength of one person’s communicative musicality can support the vitality of another’s through the application of skilful techniques that encourage an intimate, supportive, therapeutic, spirited companionship. Turning to brain science, we focus on hemispheric differences and the affective neuroscience of Jaak Panksepp. We emphasize that the psychobiological purpose of our innate musicality grows from the integrated rhythms of energy in the brain for prospective, sensation-seeking affective guidance of vitality of movement. We conclude with a Coda that recalls the philosophy of the Scottish Enlightenment, which built on the work of Heraclitus and Spinoza. This view places the shared experience of sensations of living – our communicative musicality – as inspiration for rules of logic formulated in symbols of language.

“There are certain aspects of the so-called ‘inner life’—physical or mental —which have formal properties similar to those of music—patterns of motion and rest, of tension and release, of agreement and disagreement, preparation, fulfilment, excitation, sudden change, etc. Langer (1942 , p. 228).

“The function of music is to enhance in some way the quality of individual experience and human relationships; its structures are reflections of patterns of human relations, and the value of a piece of music as music is inseparable from its value as an expression of human experience” Blacking (1995 , p.31).

“The act of musicking establishes in the place where it is happening a set of relationships, and it is in those relationships that the meaning of the act lies. They are to be found not only between those organized sounds which are conventionally thought of as being the stuff of musical meaning but also between the people who are taking part, in whatever capacity, in the performance” Small (1998 , p.9).

We present a view that places our ability to create and appreciate music at the center of what it means to be human. We argue that music is the sounds of human bodies, voices and minds – our personalities – moving in creative, story-making ways. These stories, which we want to share and listen to, are born from awareness of a complex body evolved for moving with an imaginative, future seeking mind in collaboration with other human bodies and minds. Musical stories do not need words for the creation of rich and inspiring narratives of meaning.

We adopt the word ‘musicking’ (as used above by Christopher Small) to draw attention to the embodied energy that creates music, and which moves us, emotionally and bodily. Further, we argue that music comes from the way in which knowing bodies ( Merleau-Ponty , 2012 [1945] , p. 431) prospectively explore the environment using habitual ‘patterns of action,’ which we have identified as our innate ‘communicative musicality,’ observed while infants are in intimate communication with loving caregivers ( Malloch and Trevarthen, 2009a ). In short case studies of infant interactions with micro analyses of video and audio recordings, we show communicative musicality in the timings and shapes of intersubjective vocalizations and body movements of adult and child that improvise with delight shared narratives of meaning.

Following a survey of the history of discoveries of infant abilities, we propose that the gestural narrative structures of voice and body seen as infants communicate with loving caregivers, ‘protonarrative envelopes’ of expression for ideas of activity ( Panksepp and Bernatzky, 2002 ; Panksepp and Trevarthen, 2009 ), are the building blocks of what become particular cultural instances of the art of music, and of dance, theater and other temporal arts ( Blacking, 1995 ).

As the child grows and becomes a toddler, she or he eagerly takes part in a children’s musical culture of the playground ( Bjørkvold, 1992 ). Soon more formal education with a teacher leads the way to the learning of traditional musical techniques. It is at this point that the child’s innate body vitality of communicative musicality can be encouraged and strengthened through sensitive, respectful, playful, culturally informed teaching ( Ingold, 2018 ). On the other hand, it may wither under the weight of enforced discipline for the sake of conforming to pre-existing cultural rules without attention to the initiative and pleasure of the learner’s own music-making.

The central importance of our abilities for music as part of what sustains our well-being is supported by evidence that communicative musicality strengthens emotions of social resilience to recover from mental stress and illness ( Pavlicevic, 1997 , 1999 , 2000 ). Drawing on the experience of the first author as a counselor, we argue that the strength of one person’s communicative musicality can support the vitality of another’s through the application of skilful techniques that encourage an intimate, supportive, therapeutic, spirited companionship.

Turning to brain science, focussing on hemispheric differences in performance and in response to music, and the affective neuroscience of Jaak Panksepp (1998) , we emphasize that the psychobiological purpose of our ‘muse within’ ( Bjørkvold, 1992 ) grows from the integrated rhythms of neural energy for prospective, sensation-seeking affective guidance of vitality of movement in the brain ( Goodrich, 2010 ; Stern, 2010 ).

We conclude with a Coda – an enquiry into the philosophy of the Scottish Enlightenment, which built on the work of philosophers Heraclitus and Spinoza. This view of living in community gives innate sympathy or ‘feeling with’ other humans a fundamental role within a duplex mind seeking harmony in relationships by attunement of motives ( Hutcheson, 1729 , 1755 ). It places the shared experience of sensations of living – our communicative musicality – ahead of logic formulated in symbols of language.

Music Moves US – Embodied Narratives of Movement

Small (1998) calls attention to music as intention in activity by using the verb musicking – participating as performer or listener with attention to the sounds created and the appreciation and participation by others. The compelling quality of music comes from the relationships of sounds, bodies and psyches. ‘Musicking’ points to our musical life in active ‘I-Thou’ relationships. Only in this intimacy of consciousness and its interests can we share ‘I-It’ identification and use of objects, giving things we use, including musical compositions, meaning ( Buber, 1923/1970 ).

‘I-Thou’ relationships are entered into through the body. The philosopher Merleau-Ponty (2012 [1945]) writes that “The subject only achieves his ipseity [individual personality, selfhood] by actually being a body, and by entering into the world through his body… The ontological world and body that we uncover at the core of the subject are not the world and the body as ideas ; rather, they are the world itself condensed into a comprehensive whole and the body itself as a knowing-body ” (p. 431; italics added). Musicking is knowing bodies coming alive in the sounds they make. Scores and other tools that record the product of musicking, performed or imagined, aid the retention of ideas, as semantics of language does, and they serve discussion and analysis – but they are not the same as the breathing, moving, embodied experience of human musicking ( Mithen, 2005 ).

Musicking is the expression of the sensations of what we call our communicative musicality , for the purpose of creating music that is enlivening and ‘beautiful’ ( Malloch, 1999 ; Malloch and Trevarthen, 2009a ; Trevarthen, 2015 ; Trevarthen and Malloch, 2017b ). We define communicative musicality as our innate skill for moving, remembering and planning projects in sympathy with others through time, creating an endless variety of dramatic temporal narratives in song or instrumental music. We describe this life-sharing in movement as having three components:

Pulse – a regular succession through time of discrete movements (which may, for example, be used to create sound for music, or to create movement with music – dance) using our felt sense of acting which enables the ‘future-creating’ predictive process by which a person may anticipate or create what happens next and when.

Quality – consisting of the contours of expressive vocal and body gesture, shaping our felt sense of time in movement. These contours can consist of psychoacoustic attributes of vocalizations – timbre, pitch, volume – or attributes of direction and intensity of the moving body perceived in any modality.

Narratives of individual experience and of companionship, built from sequences of co-created gestures which have particular attributes of pulse and quality that bring aesthetic pleasure ( Malloch and Trevarthen, 2009b ; Trevarthen and Malloch, 2017b ).

With music we create memorable poetic events in signs that express in sound our experience of living together in the creating vitality of ‘the present moment’ ( Stern, 2004 , 2010 ). The anthropologist and ethnologist Claude Lévi-Strauss draws attention of linguists to the structured ‘raw’ emotive power of music (beyond what words may be ‘cooked’ to say).

“In the first volume of the Mythologiques, Le Cru et le Cuit. Lévi-Strauss refers to music as a unique system of signs possessing ‘its own peculiar vehicle which does not admit of any general, extramusical use’. Yet he also allows that music has levels of structure analogous to the phonemes and sentences of language. The absence of words as the connecting level is an obvious and pertinent fact in the structuring of meaning within music as a sign system.” ( Champagne, 1990 , p. 76).

Goodrich (2010) , in her appreciation of the contribution of neuroscientists Llinás (2001) and Buzsáki (2006) to the science of the mind for skilled movement, cites Llinás’ evidence on the role of intuitive structural ‘rules,’ seen also in a musical performance.

“Llinás describes another method of keeping movement as efficient as possible: motor ‘Fixed Action Patterns’ (FAPs), distinct and complicated ‘habits’ of movement built from reflexes, habits that we develop to streamline both neural action and muscle movement. These are not entirely fixed, despite their name; they are constantly undergoing modification, adaptation, refinement, and they overlap each other… Llinás even argues that the extraordinarily precise motor control of Jascha Heifetz playing Tchaikovsky’s violin concerto is composed of highly elaborated and refined FAPs, a description most instrumentalists would find absolutely plausible” ( Goodrich, 2010 , p. 339).

As Llinás himself writes,

“Can playing a violin concerto be a FAP? Well, not all of it, but a large portion. Indeed, the unique and at once recognisable style of play Mr. Heifetz brings to the instrument is a FAP, enriched and modulated by the specifics of the concert, generated by the voluntary motor system” (Llinás, p. 136).

We add that skilled FAPs are not “composed of reflexes” as separate automatic responses. Rather they are purposeful projects that are animated to be developed imaginatively, and affectively, with exploration of their biomechanical “degrees of freedom,” as in Nikolai Bernstein’s detailed description of how a toddler learns to become a virtuoso in bipedal locomotion, which he calls The Genesis of the Biodynamical Structure of the Locomotor Act ( Bernstein, 1967 , p. 78). The testing of these locomotor acts is with an immediate and essential estimation by gut feelings ( Porges, 2011 ) of any risks or benefits, any fears or joy, they may entail within the body.

Music Reflects the Felt-Sense of our Future-Exploring Motor Intelligence

Consciousness is created as the ongoing sense of self-in-movement with which we experience and manipulate the world around us. Its origin is in our evolutionary animal past, evolved for new collaborative, creative projects, regulated between us by affective expressions of feelings of vitality from within our bodies ( Sherrington, 1955 ; Panksepp, 1998 ; Damasio, 2003 ; Mithen, 2005 ; Stern, 2010 ; Eisenberg and Sulik, 2012 ).

Using the philosopher and psychologist James (1892/1985) as a starting point for exploring the intimacy of feeling that supports and guides psychotherapy, Russell Meares (2005 , p.18), following the ‘conversational model’ of therapy developed with his collaborator psychiatrist/psychotherapist Hobson (1985) , identifies five dimensions of the self:

1. awareness which is necessary for the experience;

2. there is a shape to this inner life;

3. there is a sense of its ongoingness ;

4. our inner life has a connectedness or unity ;

5. our experience of our inner life goes on inside a virtual container which is our background emotional state and the background experience of our body .

These sensuous qualities of the experienced self are expressed in music, and in other temporal arts, as ‘the human seriousness of play’ ( Turner, 1982 ). Music, as Susan Langer says so clearly in the quote at the start of this paper, has qualities of this inner life described by Meares as shape, ongoingness and flow, connectedness and unity. The notion of music as expressive of the movements of our inner life has also been explored by music theorists, most notably Ernst Kurth (1991) . Likewise, in his book Self comes to Mind , Antonio Damasio likens all our emotion and feeling to a ‘musical score’ that accompanies other ongoing mental process ( Damasio, 2010 , p.254).

The ultimate motivation for creating music can even be traced to the cellular level. In Man On His Nature ( Sherrington, 1955 ), in a chapter entitled The Wisdom of the Body , the creator of modern neurophysiology Charles Sherrington called the coming together of communities of cells into the integrated body, nervous system and brain of a person “an act of imagination” (p. 103). Neuroscientist Rudolfo Llinás also grants subjectivity, a sense of self, to all forms of life. “Irritability [i.e., responding to external stimuli with organized, goal-directed behavior] and subjectivity, in a very primitive sense, are properties originally belonging to single cells” ( Llinás, 2001 , p.113). “Thinking”, writes Llinás (2001 , p.62), “ultimately represents movement, not just of body parts or objects in the external world, but of perceptions and complex ideas as well.”

Intrinsic to the sociability of this intelligence of movement is sensitivity for the exploration of the future, which is woven into our creation and experience of music. Karl Lashley (1951) reflecting on the evolution of animal movement, proposed that the ability to predict what might come next, and to plan the ‘serial ordering’ of separate actions, may be understood as the foundation for our logical reasoning as an individual, as it is for the grammar or syntax and prosody of our communication in language. It is essential for musicking. A restless future-seeking intelligence, with our urge to share it, inspires us to express our personalities as ‘story-telling creatures,’ who want to share, and evaluate, others’ stories ( Bruner, 1996 , 2003 ).

All animal life depends on motivated movement – the urge to explore with curiosity – to move towards food with anticipation, to move away from a predator with fear, to interact playfully with a trusted friend ( Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989 ; Panksepp and Biven, 2012 ; Bateson and Martin, 2013 ). A great achievement of modern science of the mind was the discovery by a young Russian psychologist Nikolai Bernstein of how all consciously made body movements depend upon an ‘image of the future’ ( Bernstein, 1966 , 1967 ).

Bernstein applied the new technology of movie photography to make refined ‘cyclographic’ diagrams of displacements of body parts, from which he analyzed the forces involved to fractions of a second. His findings reported in Coordination and Regulation of Movements became widely known in English translation in 1967, at the same time as video records of infant behaviors were described more accurately (see next Section The Genesis of Music in Infancy – A Short History of Discoveries), revealing their anticipatory motor control adapted for intelligent understanding of how objects may be manipulated, as well as for communication and cooperation ( Trevarthen, 1984b , 1990b ).

Our musical creativity and pleasure come from the way our body hopes to move, with rhythms and feelings of grace and biological ‘knowing’ (see Merleau-Ponty). The predicting, embodied self of a human being experiences time, force, space, movement, and intention/directionality in being. Together, these form the Gestalt of ‘vitality’ ( Stern, 2010 ), the ‘forms of feeling’ ( Hobson, 1985 ) by which we sense in ourselves and in others that movement, be that movement of the body or of a piece of music, is ‘well-done’ ( Trevarthen and Malloch, 2017b ).

The Genesis of Music in Infancy – A Short History of Discoveries

The ability to create meaning with others through wordless structured gestural narratives, that is, our communicative musicality, emerges from before birth and in infancy. From this innate musicality come the various cultural forms of music.

Any attempt to understand how human life has evolved its unique cultural habits needs to start with observing what infants know and can do. Organisms regulate the development of their lives by growing structures and processes from within their vitality, by autopoiesis that requires anticipation of adaptive functions. And they must develop and protect their abilities in response to environmental affordances and dangers, with consensuality ( Maturana and Varela, 1980 ; Maturana et al., 1995 ). Infants are ready for human cultural invention and collaboration as newly hatched birds are ready for flying – within ‘the biology of love’ ( Maturana and Varela, 1980 ; Maturana and Verden-Zoller, 2008 ). All organisms reach out in time and space to make use of the ‘affordances’ for thought and action ( Gibson, 1979 ).

Infants have no language to learn what other humans know, or what ancestors knew. But the vitality of their spontaneous communicative musicality, highly coordinated and adapted to be shared through narratives with sympathetic and playful companions, enables meaningful communication in the ‘present moment’ ( Stern, 2004 ; Figure 1 , Upper Right), which may build serviceable memories extended in space and time ( Donaldson, 1992 ).

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Inborn musicality shared in movement. Upper Left: Infant less than one hour after birth watches her mother’s tongue protrusion and imitates. Upper Right: On the day of birth, a baby in a hospital in India shares a game with a woman who moves a red ball. The baby tracks it with coordinated movements of her eyes and head, both hands and one foot. Lower: A two-month premature girl, Nasira, with her father, who holds her against his body in ‘kangaroo’ care. They exchange short ‘coo’ sounds with precisely shared rhythm. Upper Left and Upper Right: Photos for own use of second author from colleagues Vasudevi Reddy and Kevan Bundell. Part reproduced from Trevarthen (2015) , Figure 1, p. 131. Lower: Photo from Trevarthen (2008) , Figure 2, p. 22. Original spectrograph in Malloch (1999) , Figure 3, p. 37.

In this section we review changes in understanding of infant abilities over the past century which can help explain the peculiar way music has in the past been seen by some leading psychologists and linguists as a relatively insignificant epiphenomenon, learned for play, not, as we argue in this paper, a source of all talents for communication, rooted in our innate human communicative musicality of knowing bodies ( Merleau-Ponty , 2012 [1945] ) moving with prospective intuition to engage the world in company, ‘intersubjectively,’ from the start ( Zeedyk, 2006 ).

Two leading scholars in medical science and the science of child development in the past century, Freud (1923) and Piaget (1958 , 1966 ), declared that infants must be born without conscious selves conceiving an external world, and unable to adapt their movements to the expressive behavior of other people. The playful and emotionally charged behaviors of mothers and other affectionate carers were considered inessential to the young infant, who needed only responses to reflex demands for food, comfort and sleep.

Then René Spitz (1945) and Bowlby (1958) revealed the devastating effects on a child’s emotional well-being of separation from maternal care in routine hospital care with nursing directed only to respond to those reflex demands. Spitz observed that babies develop smiling between 2 and 5 months to regulate social contacts ( Spitz and Wolf, 1946 ), and he went on to study the independent will of the baby to regulate engagements of care or communication, by nodding the head for ‘yes’ or shaking for ‘no’ ( Spitz, 1957 ).

In the 1960s a major shift in understanding of the creative mental abilities of infants was inspired by a project of the educational psychologist Bruner (1966 , 1968 ), and the pediatrician Brazelton (1961 , 1979 ), who perceived that infants are gifted with sensibilities for imaginative play and ready to start cultural learning from the first weeks after birth. Supported by insights of Charles Darwin and by new findings of anthropology and animal ethology they studied infant initiatives to perceive and use objects, and they were impressed by the intimate reciprocal imitation that develops between infants and affectionate parents and caregivers who offer playful collaboration with the child’s rhythms and qualities of movement. Film studies showed that young infants make complex shifts of posture and hand gestures that are regulated rhythmically, similar to the same movements of adults ( Bruner, 1968 ; Trevarthen, 1974 ).

While this new appreciation of infant abilities was developed at the Center for Cognitive Studies at Harvard, radically transforming the ‘cognitive revolution’ that was announced there by George Miller, Noam Chomsky and Jerome Bruner in 1960, nearby at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, a project initiated by Bullowa (1979) sought evidence on the behaviors that regulate dialog before language. Bullowa used information from anthropology to draw attention to the measured dynamics of communication.

“For an infant to enter into the sharing of meaning he has to be in communication, which may be another way of saying sharing rhythm …. The problem is how two or more organisms can share innate biological rhythms in such a way as to achieve communication which can permit transmission of information they do not already share.” ( Bullowa, 1979 , p. 15, italics added).

Wanting to understand how the rhythmic flow of dialog can be shared with a child too young for speech, she pointed a way to the appreciation of the role of human communicative musicality as inspiration for the development of behaviors for carrying meaning in language – thinking and communicating in words that will be acquired to specify facts, and to describe and think about how these facts are related or may be used.

The importance of rhythm and the graceful narratives of movement displayed by infants as they communicate purposes and feelings was revealed sixty years ago by a psychobiological approach using photography and movie film, then video. Discoveries were made that challenged the theory that infants had no minds, no sense of self, and therefore no sense of others ( Zeedyk, 2006 ; Reddy, 2008 ). Most astonishing, and dismissed with derision by convinced rational mind-separate-from-body constructivists, was the finding that infants activate the many parts of their body with an exquisite sense of time, and that they can use the rhythms of expression skilfully to imitate in inter-synchrony with attentive responses from an adult (Figure 1 ).

In his work as a pediatrician, Brazelton (1961) , developing his now famous Neonatal Assessment Scale ( Brazelton, 1973 ; Brazelton and Nugent, 1995 ), accepted and encouraged the natural love mother and father felt for their new baby, and showed how appreciative the baby could be of their actions to each other and to the baby. This welcoming of the newborn as a person with intelligence and sociable impulses confirmed the parents’ belief that they could communicate feelings and interests by responding to their baby’s exquisitely timed looks, smiles, hand gestures and cooing with their own exquisitely timed gestures of voice and body. It transformed medical concern for the baby. As Brazelton declared in Margaret Bullowa’s book, “The old model of thinking of the newborn infant as helpless and ready to be shaped by his environment prevented us from seeing his power as a communicant in the early mother-father-infant interaction. To see the neonate as chaotic or insensitive provided us with the capacity to see ourselves as acting ‘on’ rather than ‘with’ him” ( Brazelton, 1979 , p.79).

New attention to newborns within hours of their delivery, with the aid of films, led to confirmation that the baby could imitate adult expressions with careful timing of movements of eyes, face, mouth and hands ( Maratos, 1973 , 1982 ; Meltzoff and Moore, 1977 ; Kugiumutzakis, 1993 ; Nagy and Molnár, 1994 , 2004 ; Nagy, 2011 ; Kugiumutzakis and Trevarthen, 2015 ; Figure 1 , Upper Left). The findings proved that the baby is born with an altero-ceptive awareness of another person’s body parts as having feelings in movement like their own proprio-ceptive ones. It also became clear that this consciousness appreciates the balance and drama of a collaborative narrative flow with shared rhythms – essential to our ability for musicking ( Trevarthen, 1974 , 1977 , 2005a ).

The pediatrician Sander, (1964 , 1975 ; republished in Sander, 2008 ) recognized that an infant and caregiver create a coherent system of actions regulated with feelings of vitality in shared time. This dynamic collaboration was also discovered by Daniel Stern when he examined recordings of a mother playing with her three-month-old twins ( Stern, 1971 ).

A stimulating contribution to this new approach came from the work of anthropologist and linguist Mary Catherine Bateson, daughter of anthropologists Gregory Bateson and Margaret Meade. In 1969 Bateson had her first child after beginning postgraduate studies at MIT with Margaret Bullowa, researching language development using statistical analysis of vocal expressions. Observing a film in Bullowa’s collection as well as the experience of rich exchanges with her own infant opened her awareness of the form and timing of communication that developed in the first 3 months, which she called ‘proto-conversation’ ( Bateson, 1971 ). She benefitted from attention to the field studies of Albert Scheflen on the stream of conversation ( Scheflen, 1972 ) and Ray Birdwhistell on body movements in natural conversation ( Birdwhistell, 1970 ).

Reviewing her work in Bullowa’s book, she said:

“… the mother and infant were collaborating in a pattern of more or less alternating, non-overlapping vocalization, the mother speaking brief sentences and the infant responding with coos and murmurs, together producing a brief joint performance similar to conversation, which I called ‘proto conversation’. The study of timing and sequencing showed that certainly the mother and probably the infant, in addition to conforming in general to a regular pattern, were acting to sustain it or to restore it when it faltered, waiting for the expected vocalization from the other and then after a pause resuming vocalization, as if to elicit a response that had not been forthcoming. These interactions were characterized by a sort of delighted, ritualized courtesy and more or less sustained attention and mutual gaze. Many of the vocalizations were of types not described in the acoustic literature on infancy, since they were very brief and faint, and yet were crucial parts of the jointly sustained performances.” ( Bateson, 1979 , p. 65).

Bateson’s work confirmed Bruner’s realization that Noam Chomsky’s hypothesis of a specific Language Acquisition Device (LAD) or innate ability of the child to construct syntax, the grammatical order of words, to formulate ideas ( Chomsky, 1965 ), paid no heed to the vital importance of a complementary ability of a parent to encourage enrichment of reference in communication with a young child, a Language Acquisition Support System (LASS). As Bruner, expressing his psychology of education, put it “the LADD needs a LASS” ( Bruner, 1983 ). Child and adult share rules of imagination for all kinds of movement, including spoken propositions.

A follower of Chomsky’s theory of the evolution of language as reasoning, Stephen Pinker, in his perhaps overly ambitiously titled How the Mind Works ( Pinker, 1997 ) claimed, “As far as biological cause and effect are concerned, music is useless.” He gave no attention to movement and time, the communication of infants, playful children, affectionate parents, the poetry of music, or Einstein’s theory of his mathematical invention as “sensations of bodily movement” ( Hadamard, 1945 ), thus misunderstanding the origins and purpose of rational discourse (see Sections Communicative Musicality and Resilience of the Human Spirit and Musical Affections of the Embodied Human Brain).

We now have evidence from many studies analyzing behaviors that demonstrate that infants show a rich spectrum of expressive movements of the upper parts of their bodies ( Trevarthen, 1984a ), not just the ‘categorical emotions’ identified by Paul Ekman ( Ekman and Friesen, 1975 ), but the ‘complex social emotions’ that Damasio (1999 , 2010 ) describes as regulators of well-being in intimate interpersonal relations, and expression of a moral personality in society – expressions of such feelings as embarrassment, shame, guilt, contempt, compassion, and admiration.

Most importantly, study of recordings reveal that modulations of timing, of rhythms, and of the flow of vitality forms shared with infants have the characteristics recognized as musical ( Trevarthen, 1990a ). These have been precisely defined by acoustic analysis of vocalizations of adult and infant in dialogs and games ( Malloch et al., 1997 ; Malloch, 1999 ; Trevarthen, 1999 ; Trehub, 2003 ).

Case Studies of Infant Musicality

We summarize here key findings related to the growth of musical abilities from studies of infant individuals that we have reported previously.

First there is evidence from a recording made by Saskia van Rees in an intensive care unit in Amsterdam ( van Rees and de Leeuw, 1993 ) that rhythms corresponding to those of human locomotion are present in vocalizations of a premature infant which are precisely coordinated with simple vocal exchanges with a caring father (Figure 1 , Lower).

The recording of a two-month premature girl with her father, who was holding her under his clothes against his body in ‘kangaroo’ care, shows that they exchange short ‘coo’ sounds, the father imitating her sounds, with precise timing based on a comfortable walking rhythm of andante – one step every 0.7 s. Father (F) and the baby Naseera (N) are equally precise in their timing, which also shows what a phonetician would recognize as a ‘final lengthening’ characteristic of a spoken phrase - when they are ready to stop the dialog the interval lengthens to 0.85 s. Following the shared phrase with its syllable-length durations, they exchange single sounds separated at 4 s intervals, the normal duration of a short spoken phrase. The recording supports our contention that even a prematurely born baby is skilled in sharing a musical pulse ( Trevarthen, 2009 ).

A recording with a blind 5-month-old girl illustrates intermodal attunement between the heard melody of a mother’s song and the proprioceptive feelings in the body of the baby of a gesturing left arm and hand (Figure 2 , Upper). The human ability to sense the shape of a melody within the body is intrinsic to our enjoyment of music as human communication ( Stern, 2010 ). Maria is totally blind and has never seen her hand. Maria and her mother were assisting in a project of Professor Gunilla Preisler in Stockholm to aid communication with blind and deaf infants.

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Communicating in the rhythm of narratives with baby songs. Upper: A five-month-old girl, who is totally blind, ‘conducts’ her mother’s singing with her left hand. The graph of her hand movements with her mother’s voice marks, with black bars, three moments in the narrative, at 4, 16, and 28 s, where the infant anticipates the mother’s voice by 0.3 s. These are times, 12 s apart, when lines of the slow lullaby commence. The arrows identify moments when there is perfect synchrony between the mother’s voice and the baby’s index finger. The circled index finger movement is 300 msec ahead of a lift in the pitch of the mother’s voice. Lower: Two infants share the poetry of action songs with their mothers. A four-month-old enjoys her mother’s singing of a song and accompanies the narrative with movements of her baby’s hand. And a six-month-old imitates clapping movements in synchrony with the song. Both songs show an iambic rhythm of short and long syllables, and rhyming vowels, ‘bear’ with ‘there’ and ‘well’ with ‘bell,’ which the babies imitate with their voices. (Upper: Analyses of video from Professor Gunilla Preisler, University of Stockholm. Previously published in Trevarthen (1999) and Schögler and Trevarthen (2007) . Lower: From Trevarthen (2015) , modified from Figure 4, p.137.)

While her baby is lying down during bottle feeding, the mother sings two baby songs including “Mors Lille Olle,” well-known throughout Scandinavia. It was not realized until later when the video was viewed that Maria was ‘conducting’ the melodies with delicate expressive movements of her left hand, while the right hand was making unrelated movements, stroking her body. When Professor of Music at Edinburgh University, Nigel Osborne, saw the film he said, “Yes she is conducting using the conventional movements of a professional conductor, describing a phrase with a sweeping movement, pointing up for a higher pitch, and dropping her wrist at the close of a verse – and she is making the movements with some anticipation.” Microanalysis supported what he observed. At certain points in the course of the melody Maria’s finger moves 300 milliseconds before the mother’s voice. She knows the song well, and leads the ‘performance’ ( Trevarthen, 1999 ; Schögler and Trevarthen, 2007 ).

Although blind, Maria knows the feelings of anticipated movement of her hand, and uses them to sense and share the human vitality dynamics in her mother’s voice. This kinematic sensibility was identified by Olga Maratos in her pioneering research in imitation as foundational for the ability of a young infant to reproduce another person’s expression seen or heard ( Maratos, 1973 , 1982 ). Indeed, vocal perception, detecting the modulation of pitch and timing in an adult’s voice sounds, develops much faster than vocal production. The infant may be tracking sound with reference to the kinesics of the fastest and most complex gestural movements of her hands.

When taking part in a nursery song, infants demonstrate sensitivity for melodic phrase structure, attending to the rhyming vowels at the ends of lines, and by 5 months the infant can vocalize a matching vowel in synchrony with the mother ( Trevarthen, 2008 ). For our final example, the application of acoustic analysis with observations on gestural behaviors of infants in the middle of the first year, suggests that melodic patterns, common to different cultures, define four-line verses ( Imberty, 2000 ), with a pattern of Introduction, Development, Climax and Resolution, identified in proto-conversations with two-month-olds (Figure 2 , Lower, and Figure 3 ), and we note similarities with the sections recognized in classical Roman rhetoric or speech-making – exordium, narratio, confutatio , and confirmatio . In spite of very different conventions in musical performances in different communities, a parent, or a child, wanting to share the pleasure of songs and action games with a baby, naturally adopts the intuitive formula of a poetic verse to share a story of body movement.

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Stories of changing pitch in the singing voice. These examples illustrate the narrative phases that infants respond to, which are found in infant directed speech or baby songs in different languages. (A) A proto-conversation of a mother with a 6-week-old. The baby shares the development, climax and resolution of the narrative, with vocalizations close to middle C. (B) A slow lullaby, of 26 s, has four-line stanzas, with rhyming vowels. (C) A more animated song of 18 s shows the same poetic organization with undulations of pitch and vowels rhyming between first and second lines and between final words of each verse. (A) Original in Malloch (1999) , Figure 5, p. 41. (B) From Trevarthen (2015) , a portion of Figure 4, p. 137, and Trevarthen (2016) , Figure 3, p. 10. Original in Malloch (1999) , Figure 10, p. 49. (C) Original in Trevarthen (1999) , Figure 2, p. 183.

Lastly, we present the work of Katerina Mazokopaki, a developmental psychologist who is a pianist and teacher of piano playing. She made a study of babies in Crete with her professor, Giannis Kugiumutzakis, an expert in analysis of imitative games with newborns ( Mazokopaki and Kugiumutzakis, 2009 ). The babies were left alone in a familiar place at home amusing themselves. Then a recording of a Greek baby song came on. Between 3 and 10 months old they all reacted in the same way. First they looked surprised; then they looked about as if someone had come into the room; and finally they smiled with delight and started performing with the music, inspired by the pulse and melody, joining the music with their different abilities to dance and sing (Figure 4 ).

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Katerina Mazokopaki’s baby dancers: Upper Left: Georgos, 3.5 months lying on a comfortable bed, responds with a big smile and gestures of hands and feet to accompany the music. Upper Right: Katerina, 9 months, smiles, bounces and extends her arms to ‘fly’ into action. Lower Left: Panos, 9 months, smiles his greeting, then beats the floor rhythmically with his right hand. Lower Right: Anna, 10 months, standing in her cot, smiles and starts dancing vigorously, swinging her bottom. All four sing with the music (Photos supplied to the CT).

Communicative Musicality and Education into the Culture of Music

Mastery of a musical culture, and of language, starts with the intuitive vocal interactions between caregiver and infant ( Vygotsky, 1966 ). Our innate communicative musicality is the ‘raw material’ for cultural forms of music and the rules of grammar and syntax. A child makes stories in sound as an active participant whose pride to belong to the rich musical traditions of society propels them into learning and creating. This is the cultivation of communicative musicality to music, from innate self-expression to cultural practice and a musical identity ( MacDonald et al., 2002 ; MacDonald and Miell, 2004 ). It is brought to life, as language is, with the enthusiastic support of more experienced companions ( Bruner, 1983 ).

The desire for cultural participation is evident in informal learning in which children’s own musical culture grows from the vitality of The Muse Within ( Bjørkvold, 1992 ). It is nurtured from the music, live and recorded, that the child hears all around and contributes to spontaneously, along with the invention of talking and verse-making with playmates, often accompanied by rhythmic stamping, hopping and jumping ( Chukovsky, 1963 ). Our earliest shared signing of communicative musicality in infancy becomes dialogic ‘musical babbling’ from around 2 months old ( Trevarthen, 1990a ). Already at 2 months the infant is learning the cultural gestures and preferences that become the tools through which cultural meaning will be created and exchanged ( Moog, 1976 ; Tafuri and Hawkins, 2008 ). Lullabies are sung more often in cultures that value quiet infants, playsongs in cultures that value lively enthusiasm in infants ( Trehub and Trainor, 1998 ). A musical ‘proto-habitus’ is created ( Gratier and Trevarthen, 2008 ).

The infant is born ready to interact and discover her musical culture. Hearing responds to musical sounds from the third trimester of pregnancy ( Busnel et al., 1992 ), and infants can recognize music they heard before birth ( Hepper, 1991 ). They recognize musical contours and rhythmic patterns ( Trehub and Hannon, 2006 ), and ‘dance’ to music before they are one year old ( Zentner et al., 2010 ). Infant-inclusive singing is preferred, like infant-inclusive speaking 1 .

From about 3 months of age in many (probably all) cultures, mothers start to sing baby songs ( Trevarthen, 1986 ) to their infants. In a previous publication the second author summarized their characteristics:

(1) “The beat is clear and precisely modulated; usually in the andante range near moderato (90-100 beats per minute).

(2) Often the beat is marked by clapping, or by regular movements of the baby’s body.

(3) The mother sings with a clear melodic tone varying her pitch in a treble range and making rises and falls to make a simple development of the ‘emotional narrative’.

(4) Songs commonly have a clear stanza form — usually 4 lines, each of 3 or 4 beats, making a verse (which therefore lasts 8-15 seconds).

(5) Especially in the last line, well controlled musical tricks varying the beat ( rubato , sforzando, ritardando, accelerando ) are used to mark a climax and resolution.” ( Trevarthen, 1989 , p. 96).

More recognizable musical forms grow with the spontaneous singing of young children as they play alone or with others ( Barrett, 2011 ), practicing their musical craft. The Norwegian musicologist Jon-Roar Bjørkvold (1992) collected and studied the songs of 4–7-year-olds in three kindergartens in Oslo. He observed how they gave voice to emotion, conveyed information, and established relationships through learning and creating their own children’s musical culture. He identified two types of children’s singing – ‘egocentric’ for private pleasure, which, as the child matures, gives way to more social or ‘communicative’ music making.

As young children mature so they use their voice with a singing kind of expression in progressively more ‘symbolic’ ways. Fluid/Amorphous Songs “evolve in a completely natural way from the infant’s babbling as part of its first playful experiments with voice and sound. This type of spontaneous song, with its fanciful glissandi, micro-intervals, and free rhythms, is quite different from what we adults traditionally identify as song.” ( Bjørkvold, 1992 , p.65). Song Formulas , such as teasing songs, are symbolic forms for communicating and they flourish after the child begins to play with peers, typically at two or three. Elements of musically more complex Standard Songs are picked up from play with adults and hearing them sing, and are adapted to fit what the child is doing. This progressive ‘ritualisation’ of vocal creativity clarifies the adaptive motives for learning to sing, and how they express increasing narrative imagination for sharing ideas in culturally specific ways ( Gratier and Trevarthen, 2008 ; Eckerdal and Merker, 2009 ), paralleling the way language is mastered ( Chukovsky, 1963 ).

All through the development of children’s singing, repetition and variation, basic tools of any piece of music (for example, see Ockelford, 2017 ), are primary features as children explore the possibilities of musical form. Repetition and variation between the vocalizations of infant and caregiver feature from the very first shared vocalizations, regulating feelings in social interactions ( Malloch, 1999 ). Later, the growing child will continue to play with how music can convey affect and change their own and others’ mood, the four-part structure of Introduction, Development, Climax and Resolution, identified above in the structure of a proto-conversation, becoming the basis of large scale musical works, as well as verbal argument (for example, see the sections of classical rhetoric).

How the child’s spontaneous musicality, as it grows in group practice without formal training ( MacDonald and Miell, 2004 ), is received by the surrounding educational culture, is a vital ingredient in the child’s emerging ‘musical identity.’ Musical identity and self-efficacy or mastery of skill in music making inform each other, in reciprocal relationship. A child who sees themselves as a competent musician may attempt to learn a difficult piece of music, and their success at performing this piece will further bolster their sense of competence. And the way a child is welcomed into their musical culture is of vital importance as to whether this child thrives playfully with the musical tools at her disposal, developing her skill in the use of these tools, or shrinks away in disinterest because her own intrinsic musicality is not being heard or valued. If education does its job well, with the child as collaborative artist and thinker ( Trevarthen et al., 2018 ), our rich inner narrative of affective life, generated with our prospective awareness of body movement, is expressed in our social group to create a life-affirming, inclusive culture of shared artful rituals that celebrate the aesthetic grace and moral graciousness of joy in performance ( Frank and Trevarthen, 2012 ; Trevarthen and Bjørkvold, 2016 ). For example, the InCanto project ( Tafuri and Hawkins, 2008 ) is a wonderful example of infants’ and parents’ being encouraged to have their expression of music cultivated in such a way that the infant grows into a child who shows greater ability to sing in tune, a greater range of musical expression, and overall more enthusiasm for music participation.

Problems from introducing an emphasis on enforced cultural learning too early are demonstrated by Bjørkvold (1992) who studied the musical games of children in Oslo, Moscow, St Petersburg, and Los Angeles where educational, cultural, social and political practices are very different. In all three countries children showed spontaneous musicality, but in the nations of Russia and the US, where formal training in music was given greater value than it was in Norway, he found reduced spontaneous music making. He insists, “It is critically important for children to master spontaneous singing, for it is part of the common code of child culture that gives them a special key to expression and human growth” ( Bjørkvold, 1992 , p. 63). A comparable inhibitory effect of conventions of schooling has been recorded on the spontaneous expression of religious feelings and spirituality in the early years ( Hay and Nye, 1998 ). These innate sources of human imagining in collaborative, moral ways give value and meaning to the later cultivation of advanced cultural ideas and skills ( Valiente et al., 2012 ).

The importance of valuing both the child’s innate musical creativity and introducing a child into his musical culture so that he may thrive within it and contribute to it can be conceptualized as a balance between two educational necessities – providing a social environment where a child’s own skills and abilities are nurtured, and a place where training is provided into the ways of a particular culture ( Rogoff, 2003 ). Both build enthusiasm for cultural participation. This balance has been presented by Bowman (2012) and others as a consideration of two Latin roots for the English word ‘education.’ One, educare , means to train or to mold. Its motivation is the initiation of a person into cultural conventions, without which a person is left unable to live effectively within a particular culture, using its tools to communicate. The other, educere , means to lead out, or draw out. Without this more responsive nurturing, the person is left unable to engage with situations and solve problems not yet imagined. Their ability for creativity is compromised. These very different concepts of what education means are often experienced in schooling as being in tension, with educare often winning out, leaving the child with dry knowledge rather than living abilities supported by their own innate skills.

We propose that teachers and students of music at all levels learn how best to do their work by deliberately invoking the rhythms of the student’s innate creative vitality while demonstrating cultural conventions that make rich use of this talent ( Flohr and Trevarthen, 2008 ). Infants and toddlers make imaginative musical play in affectionate friendships with parents or peers ( Custodero, 2009 ); primary school children build relationships with the invention of stories in groups with free instrumental play and dance ( Fröhlich, 2009 ); and an advanced music student is assisted to master their instrument through their teacher encouraging their playing to be like a dance representing a narrative, rich in expressive feelings ( Rodrigues et al., 2009 ). In all instances the motives of the learner, and how they may change with development of the body and experiences gained, are of crucial importance ( Bannan and Woodward, 2009 ; Ingold, 2018 ). As with all education, the success of teaching depends on recognition of how children’s ‘zest for learning’ ( Whitehead, 1929 ; Dewey, 1938 ) changes with age and the development of body and mind.

We end this section with a quote from Bowman on the broader role of music in education. In times where the arts are often considered of marginal importance in education, it talks to the richness of our engagement with music in nurturing all learning experiences:

“The distinctive educational and developmental potential of music lies, I submit, in dynamic, bodily, and social natures, and distinctly ethical, responsive, and responsible kinds of know-how these afford. Practical knowledge is action embedded knowledge, quite distinct from theoretical knowledge and technical know-how. It is a kind of character-based sense of how best to proceed in situations where best courses of action cannot be determined by previous ones. This ability to discern the right course of action in novel, dynamic situations is precisely the kind of human asset required in today’s rapidly changing world. And musical engagements may, under the right circumstances, nurture this capacity in ways unmatched by any other human endeavour.” ( Bowman, 2012 , p. 31).

Communicative Musicality and Resilience of the Human Spirit

As Daniel Stern has written ( Stern, 2010 ), the human body has a rich range of gestural ‘forms of vitality’ – we move in musical ways. And within each actor there is both ‘self-sensing’ and ‘other-sensing’ of the degree of grace, or biological efficiency ( Bernstein, 1967 ) and hopefulness ( Trevarthen and Malloch, 2017a ) in the gestural narratives of our projects. These qualities of vitality, or well-being, transmitted to others, become the qualities of relationships and social activities – their moral values ( Kirschner and Tomasello, 2010 ; Narvaez, 2014 ; Trainor and Cirelli, 2015 ). They convey relational feelings for the degree of consensuality or sharing of expression in moving. Effort to manage the grace and morality of movements can be cultivated to assist well-being of those whose actions are confused or fearful - that is, the making or ‘poetry’ (from the Greek poiein , to make) of their movements may be enhanced to provide responsive and relational care or therapy ( Hobson, 1985 , ch3; Stern, 2000 , p. xiv; Osborne, 2009b ; Meares, 2016 ).

At times our healthy ability for graceful gesturing is met with circumstances that do not allow it to be expressed with its natural healthy vitality. For example, failure to gain a sympathetic appreciation of their musicality can cause an infant to express withdrawal and distress ( Murray and Trevarthen, 1985 ). Instead of joyful pride in sharing play they show sadness and shame ( Trevarthen, 2005b ). However, an infant’s communicative musicality can also be expressive of resilience and determination.

In the example presented in Figure 5A we see a consistent rigidity of expression and a lack of self-confident invention on the part of a mother suffering from BPD (borderline personality disorder). She repeats the same up-and-down vocal gesture again and again, with almost no vocal participation on the part of the infant. Where the infant does participate (shown by vocalizations with either a square or circle around them), the infant appears to be setting up the possibility for a dialog – vocalizing exactly on the ‘bar-line’ (bar 5, shown by a square) and then around the mother’s pitch (shown by a circle). Indeed, the infant’s vocalizations persuade the mother out of her repetitiveness – the mother momentarily takes notice of her infant and responds to her infant’s conversational offering by ceasing her unresponsive repetition and vocalizing once more at the infant’s pitch. But the dialog almost immediately breaks down, and the mother returns to her stereotypical, repetitive vocal gesturing.

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Voice modulations that express emotions of relating in a psychological disorder, and in relational therapy. (A) A pitch plot of a mother suffering from BPD (borderline personality disorder) speaking to her infant. She repeats the same phrase with monotonous intonation, making a slight reaction to critically timed sounds made by the infant, but cannot establish shared affective engagement. (B,C) Changes in the prosody of a client’s speaking in communication with a therapist, before and after a therapeutic change, with improvement in self-confidence. (A) Original in Gratier and Apter-Danon (2009) , Figure 14.5, page 319. Reproduced with permission. (B,C) Originals in Malloch (2017) , Figures 4.1 and 4.2, p.72. Reproduced with permission.

As well as showing inflexible, ‘non-graceful’ behavior of a mother who is suffering from BPD, this example also shows the resilience and hope of the infant in the face of her mother’s rigid communicative style. This will of our communicative musicality, evident from the earliest of interactions with infants ( Papoušek, 1996 ), is utilized in therapies that employ in-the-moment intersubjective interactions as means for healing ( Stern et al., 1998 ; Stern, 2000 , p. xiv). This immediately responsive interaction may be through talking, through music-making, through dance, or touch. The common element is an individual who is highly skilled in attuning to nuances of interpersonal timing and gesture, and who aims to lead back to health another whose personal mindedness of time-in-the-body has become compromised through hardship, suffering, or biological disruption, perhaps leading to a sense of isolation and misunderstanding ( Trevarthen and Malloch, 2000 ). The therapist joins with the person who needs help, leading them back to health and wellbeing through their own therapeutic sense of the ‘minute particulars’ in that moment of meeting ( Hobson, 1985 , part 2).

Olga Maratos, a developmental psychologist who pioneered recognition of the ability of young infants to imitate expressions of an attentive and supportive adult ( Maratos, 1982 ) took part in the establishment in Athens of a residential school for children with autism, called Perivolaki , meaning ‘a little garden.’ It is a day-care center with facilities in beautiful surroundings that invite playful out-door activities, games with toys and creative shared occupations such as listening to and making music. They use the psychoanalytic concept of ‘transference’ of feelings or ‘unconscious desires’ to encourage sensitive intimate and consistent relations, each child having a trusting relationship with a particular member of the staff. Stability of activities is maintained, with close engagement with the parents, who are seen weekly during the first two years of the child’s stay at Perivolaki and every fortnight thereafter. The average length of stay is 4–5 years ( Maratos, 1998 ).

Olga explains that staff are trained to observe the children, think about them and discuss their behavior at staff meetings “with a view to understanding whether their autistic behaviour is defensive, refusing interaction and relations because they don’t make sense for them or because they are painful, or whether there is a pervasive lack of motivation for relating and communicating. We find both conditions present, at different times, in all our children” ( Maratos, 1998 , p. 206). This approach, adapting a psychoanalytic treatment which avoids diagnosis of the cause of the disorder, leads to a form of active and intimate ‘relational therapy,’ which does not rely on verbal formulation of anxieties and lack of trust.

Affect attunement has been defined as qualities of vocal and body gesture that carry meaning in parent–infant communication – it is, “the performance of behaviours that express the quality of feeling of a shared affect state, but without imitating the exact behavioural expression of the inner state” ( Stern, 1985 , p. 142). This largely unconscious ‘recasting’ of events is necessary to “shift the focus of attention to what is behind the behaviour, to the quality of feeling that is being shared” ( Stern, 1985 , p. 142). We say the relationship is now one of ‘companionship,’ a word from Latin meaning ‘to break bread with’ and defined here as the wish to be with an other for a mutually beneficial ‘inner’ purpose, apart from reasons of immediate survival, procreation or material gain. Companionship involves exchanging affect through sharing the quality or virtue of impulses of motivation, which is the original rich meaning of ‘sympatheia’ in Greek ( Trevarthen, 2001 ).

The therapeutic relationship, even in talking therapies focussed on what the language says about feelings, is underpinned by the manner or graciousness of our gestural exchanges – whether those qualities are carried in vocal prosody or bodily movements ( Stern, 2000 , p. xiv; Malloch, 2017 ). It is about the direct and desired sharing of feelings in human vitality. Stern (2010) writes eloquently on the importance of listening for the forms of vitality (the forms of feeling as Hobson, 1985 , called them) that are being expressed through prosody – “without the dynamic vitality features of the intention-unfolding process we would not experience a vital human being behind the words that are being said” ( Stern, 2010 , p.124). He also writes of the use of metaphors as carriers of images of our in-the-moment vitality (also see Hobson, 1985 , ch.4). For example, in the work of Stephen, the first author, as a therapist, here is the text of an exchange between him and a client discussing the client’s sense that his life has never measured up to his notion of what a successful life should look like:

Client: My father thought I was stupid. He’d call me ‘stupid boy.’ Therapist: The way you said that – perhaps a sort of exhaustion and emptiness – reminds me of the tide going out in a bay. Client: Empty… like something’s slowly leaving.

Here the focus on the vitality that the client associates with his father’s dismissing statement leads to a new experience to explore therapeutically – something is slowly leaving. Further discussion focussed on the experience of what ‘slowly leaving’ feels like. When the therapist works like this, “we move from an enquiry about intentions, means, and goal states to an enquiry about processes of creation, emerging, and becoming” ( Stern, 2010 , p.126). We move from distanced recollection or speculation to life in the present moment ( Stern, 2004 ).

The prosody of the client’s voice sometimes sums-up the therapeutic change itself. In the example below of Stephen’s work a client is talking of an emerging ‘new me’ in contrast to an ‘old me.’ 2 The ‘old me’ was marked with ‘a lack of self-respect.’ ‘I blame myself when things go wrong, I believe I’m not working hard enough.’ The voice is drone-like, body hardly moving. Figure 5B shows a four-second section of a pitch plot of ‘old me’ voice.

After describing ‘old me’ the client’s body relaxed, they looked up from the floor, hands lifted from their lap, the volume of their voice increased, its pitch lifted, and they began talking of ‘new me.’ ‘New me is more rational about life. This part says, “Well, I was uncommunicative this morning – that’s all right, that’s OK. That’s just the way I was. Doesn’t make me a bad person. Other times I communicate really well!” Figure 5C is a four-second pitch plot of ‘new me.’ The shift in the vitality of the musicality is clear. Stephen felt the distinct difference in the vitality of the two “me’s” of the client, and continued the session exploring how the new “me” might express itself in the world (see Malloch, 2017 , for further discussion of communicative musicality in therapy).

The role of our communicative musicality in supporting our wellbeing lies at the very heart of the practice of using music therapeutically. Music therapy covers a wide range of ways of using musical experiences – stories in sound – to heal and improve people’s lives. The Australian Music Therapy Association defines Music Therapy as: “a research-based practice and profession in which music is used to actively support people as they strive to improve their health, functioning and wellbeing.” It is the compassionate use of music to engage another emotionally, interpersonally, cognitively, and culturally. “Music is therapeutic because it attunes to the essential efforts that the mind makes to regulate the body, both in its inner neurochemical, hormonal and metabolic processes, and in its purposeful engagements with the objects of the world, and with other people” ( Trevarthen and Malloch, 2000 ). This is particularly so during improvisational music therapy, where the therapist supports the client towards change – greater integration of experience and freedom in communication ( Pavlicevic, 2000 , ch.6; also see Malloch et al., 2012 , on the effectiveness of improvisational music therapy with neonates).

However, the practice of music therapy is more than the therapeutic use of preverbal protomusic, however, important this is. Reflecting our discussion above on music and education, music therapy is also making use of the cultural forms of our musicality, and the power these cultural forms have within our psyche (for example, Donald, 1991 ; MacDonald et al., 2002 ; Stern, 2010 ).

A relationship between our communicative musicality and our culturally made music for the practice of music therapy is proposed by Pavlicevic and Ansdell (2009) . They emphasize the peculiarly musical relationship established within music therapy practice – that is, that the cultural elaboration of communicative musicality relates to our communal, social lives. Music therapy engages our shared communicative musicality, and welcomes us into the shared cultural, communal experience of musicking, using the tools of a particular cultural type of music – one of many musics in the world ( Stige, 2002 ).

Thus, part of the reason music therapy ‘works’ is its invitation for cultural collaboration – we exercise what has been called our ‘deep social mind’ ( Whiten, 2000 ) following particular cultural forms. This wish to learn the forms of culture, our ‘conformal motive’ ( Merker, 2009 ), comes to life within an environment where we sense our communicative gestures are being valued by another or others through the creation of shared narratives of vitality forms ( Stern, 2010 ). We feel ourselves to be both a companion in our shared narrative of communicative embodied gestures and a companion in a particular shared cultural collaboration. It is within this dual companionship that our deep yearning to belong is met and satisfied, and where healing can occur.

Musical Affections of the Embodied Human Brain

In our opinion brain science has been most insightful into the nature of the self and what makes us human, and how we share the joy and pains of life, when it investigates ‘Primary Process’ emotional guidance of brain growth for regulation of vitality in body movement and its ‘seeking’ awareness ( Hess, 1954 ; Solms and Panksepp, 2012 ). It offers us insight when it investigates how experiences develop by generating expectations of well-being in companionship and by enriching it with cultural meaning ( Trevarthen, 1990b ). It shows that differences in the rate of development of cognitive processes in left and right hemisphere at different ages are caused by different affections ( Thatcher et al., 1987 ; Chiron et al., 1997 ), from which arise correlations with musical behaviors and other creative forms of play, as well as Piagetian stages of rational mastery of the body and objects it uses, and development of language.

The neuroscientist Jaak Panksepp, who studied the emotions of mammals who do not cultivate music, but who use patterns of movement including signs with sound to regulate their social lives in ways that anticipate our richer experience of the sounds of our movement using the tools of culture, offers insight into why we are musical beings ( Panksepp and Bernatzky, 2002 ). And in a recent synopsis he agrees that mastery of language in early years depends on the sense of purpose we share in musical ways:

“Human languages are coaxed into the brain, initially by the melodic intonations of motherese by which emotional communication becomes the vehicle for propositional thought.” ( Panksepp et al., 2012 , p. 11).

Like the brain of any animal, the human brain grows to represent and regulate a body form in movement ( Trevarthen, 1980 , 2001 , 2004 ). And from even before birth, the self-formation of a personal self in the brain of the fetus is led by manifestations of movement.

“The first generalized movements occur in week 8 ( de Vries et al., 1984 ), but already in week 5 monoamine transmission pathways grow from the brainstem to animate the primordial cerebral hemispheres. Key components of the Emotional Motor System (hypothalamus, basal ganglia and amygdala) are in place when the neocortex is unformed.” ( Trevarthen, 2001 , p. 26).

After the baby is born and seeks intimate communication of all motives with a parent, the affective system remains as the director of learning and appreciation of what is gained by new awareness.

Psychiatrist and literary scholar Iain McGilchrist in The Master and His Emissary ( McGilchrist, 2009 ) has presented a brilliant review of behavioral and brain research, and a clear conception of complementary consciousness in the two cerebral hemispheres. “Music”, he writes, “being grounded in the body, communicative of emotion, implicit, is a natural expression of the nature of the right hemisphere” (p. 72).

McGilchrist’s research leads him to the political view that we are living in a society that grants too much power to the special refined perceptuo-motor and scientific skills of the left brain, while failing to appreciate how the right brain gives purpose and value to all that we do, thus pointing to the importance of closer cooperation between science and the fundamental values of the humanities. He draws on anthropological information about the universal principles of social understanding at very different levels of technical ability and manufacture, and on the importance in all social groups and cultures of musical performances, which he concludes, from a wide range of evidence, that music evolved before language and contributed to the formulation of its syntax and prosody (see Sachs, 1943 ).

Music, he says, was not,

“an irrelevant spin-off from something with more of a competitive cutting edge – namely, language…. rather the reverse. If language evolved later, it looks like it evolved from music…. Rousseau in the eighteenth century, von Humboldt in the nineteenth century and Jespersen in the twentieth, have thought it likely that language developed from music…. That we could use non-verbal means, such as music, to communicate is, in any case, hardly surprising. The shock comes partly from the way we in the West view music: we have lost the sense of the central position that music once occupied in communal life, and still does in most parts of the world today…. We might think of music as an individualistic, even solitary experience, but that is rare in the history of the world.” (p. 104).

And he quotes neurologist Oliver Sacks, who said:

“This primal role of music is to some extent lost today, when we have a special class of composers and performers, and the rest of us are often reduced to passive listening. One has to go to a concert, or a church or a musical festival, to recapture the collective excitement and bonding of music. In such a situation, there seems to be an actual binding of nervous systems, the unification of an audience by a veritable ‘neurogamy’ (to use a word favoured by early Mesmerists) ( Sacks, 2006 , p. 2528).

Twenty-six years before McGilchrist published his book, the anthropologist Victor Turner, famous for his book From Ritual to Theatre , drew on knowledge of the different functions of the hemispheres to identify play with a collaboration between them, in an article entitled “Play and drama: The horns of a dilemma”:

“Current ideas about differences between the left and right hemispheres of the brain provide a basis for speculating about the nature of play. Play encompasses both the rationality and order of the left hemispheric orientation, and the improvisation and creativity of the right. But play also transcends these oppositions, running rings about them as it encircles the brain’s consciousness” ( Turner, 1983 , p. 217).

Good, beautiful and enjoyable, music is created out of poetic play. Indeed, we play music ( Trevarthen and Malloch, 2017b ).

A sense of time in the mind is the fabric from which movements of all kinds are woven into ambitious projects that value elegance with efficiency. It is a manifestation of the ‘biochronology’ that is essential to the vitality of all forms of life ( Osborne, 2017 ). In Rhythms of the Brain , György Buzsáki (2006) presents a wealth of evidence that the brain functions as a coherent rhythmic system, always in synch., and with a rich array of rhythms that are organized to collaborate.

“At the physiological level, oscillators do a great service for the brain: they coordinate or ‘synchronize’ various operations within and across neuronal networks. Syn (meaning same) and chronos (meaning time) together make sure that everyone is up to the job and no one is left behind, the way the conductor creates temporal order among the large number of instruments in an orchestra” ( Buzsáki, 2006 , p. viii).

From Panksepp and Trevarthen (2009) , p. 114:

“Music is performed with the measure of expressive movements in time, and with tensions created by combining rhythms ( Osborne, 2009a ). The ‘architecture’ and ‘narration’ of moving in psychological time is also displayed with emotional qualities related to vital functions of the body ( Trevarthen, 2009 ). These psychobiological elements of vitality are charted in three bands or ranges of physical or scientific time: (1) for the felt and imagined ‘extended present’ (from 10 seconds to years); (2) for the conscious ‘psychological present’ ( Stern, 2004 ), with its serially ordered steps of motor control coupled to the physiological rhythms of breathing and variations in heart rate (0.3 to 7 seconds); and (3) for ‘reflex experiences’ and ‘just noticeable differences’ too fast to be regulated by movements that are prospectively controlled in awareness (5 to 200 milliseconds). (For detail and the sources of this description see Trevarthen, 1999 ).

The time of musical narrative, which Imberty (2000) calls the macrostructure or ‘story-without-words’ of music, is related to the times of expressive behavior that form ‘protonarrative envelopes’ of intuitive vocal and gestural play between infants and their mothers ( Stern, 1985 , 1995 ; Malloch, 1999 ). The period corresponding to a stanza or verse of 20 to 40 seconds may be manifested in the brain, as gamma waves or parasympathetic cycles, which control autonomic functions of the heart and breathing. It continues to be active through sleep to produce fluctuating rates of breathing and heartbeat, as well as electrical activity of the cerebral cortex that might be related to the rehearsal and consolidation of memories in dreaming ( Delamont et al., 1999 ). In wakefulness the narrative cycle is charged and modulated for intersubjective meaning with the ‘microtonal’ and ‘microtemporal’ variations of emotion that express urgency and facility in skilled control of moving within the voice of a singer or the playing fingers of an instrumental performer, and in the hearing of a listener ( Imberty, 1981 , 2000 ; Gabrielsson and Juslin, 1996 ; Juslin, 1997 , 2001 ; Kühl, 2007 ; Osborne, 2009a ). Music can assist the synchronization of physiological functions of respiration and heart activity and bring improvement in locomotor activity, and it can improve cognitive and memory processes by brain synchronization.”

Rhythmic co-ordination by the Intrinsic Motive Pulse (IMP) of the brain holds body movements together in composition of intentions and experiences ( Trevarthen, 1999 , 2016 ). It is the medium for all shared experiences and purposes, and for the convivial vitality of music making.

CODA: The Philosophy of Human Vitality

In her review of the role of movement and sense of time in the creation of intelligence, Barbara Goodrich, as a philosopher, traces a history of ideas supporting the view that consciousness is founded on emotions for agency, which we argue are the sine qua non for music. In opposition to the “implicit philosophical presuppositions inherited from the canon of Plato, Aristotle, Descartes, and Kant, e.g., that consciousness is self-reflective, passive, and timeless,” she proposes a natural science view.

“Western philosophy, however, also includes what might be described as a counter-tradition—and one that is more compatible with empirical biological science than the usual canon. Heraclitus, Spinoza, Schopenhauer, Nietzsche, and especially the 20th century French philosopher and psychologist, Merleau-Ponty, all anticipated aspects of Llinás’s and Buszáki’s approaches… sketching out a notion of consciousness emerging from motility, and generating new hypotheses for neurophysiological research.” ( Goodrich, 2010 , p. 331).

We have argued that music comes from this very foundation of consciousness in motivated motility, and we underline the importance of a philosophy that acknowledges the motives and feelings of our life, as well as the intelligence we show in relating to persons, other life forms, and objects in our environment, by recalling the achievements of the philosophers of the Scottish Enlightenment – Hutcheson, Hume, Smith and Reid. In line with Goodrich’s “counter tradition,” their work anticipates the new understanding of the human BrainMind pioneered by Panksepp and Damasio, which gives primary importance to feelings of vitality in movement, and to emotions that express positive and negative affections in sympathetic communication. This is the science of communicative musicality which underpins the music we create and enjoy.

The Scottish philosophers of the 18th Century, led by Francis Hutcheson, held that relationships and social life depend upon a universal human capacity for “innate sympathy,” which generates a conscience, a sense of beauty, a public ‘common sense’ that values happiness and is disturbed by misery, and a moral sense that perceives virtue or vice in ourselves or others ( Hutcheson, 1729 , 1755 ; Hume, 1739–1740 ).

Smith (1759) in his Theory of Moral Sentiments took ‘sympathy’ to designate any kind of ‘moving and feeling with,’ whether motivated positively or negatively, and including posturing and acting in the same expressive way as another’s body (cf. the work of Stern on ‘affect attunement’ quoted earlier), and he also imagined experiences of relating and being sensed, as, for example, interrogating one’s conscience.

“How selfish soever man may be supposed, there are evidently some principles in his nature, which interest him in the fortune of others, and render their happiness necessary to him, though he derives nothing from it except the pleasure of seeing it.”

“Sympathy… may…, without much impropriety, be made use of to denote our fellow-feeling with any passion whatever.” Part I – Of the Propriety of Action; Section I – Of the Sense of Propriety, Chapter I – Of Sympathy.

He examined his conscience to understand being a person in relations.

“When I endeavour to examine my own conduct, when I endeavour to pass sentence upon it, and either to approve or condemn it, it is evident that, in all such cases, I divide myself, as it were, into two persons; and that I, the examiner and judge, represent a different character from that other I, the person whose conduct is examined into and judged of. The first is the spectator, whose sentiments with regard to my own conduct I endeavour to enter into, by placing myself in his situation, and by considering how it could appear to me, when seen from that particular point of view’. The second is the agent, the person whom I properly call myself, and of whose conduct, under the character of a spectator, I was endeavouring to form some opinion.”

This picture of a duplex mind regulated by motives of sympathy anticipates the distinction made by William James in 1892 and by Martin Buber in 1923 between a fundamental “I-Thou” state of awareness and the objective “I-It” relations with the physical word we acquire in communication.

Otteson, joint professor of philosophy and economics, and chair of the Philosophy Department, at Yeshiva University, and adjunct Professor of Economics at New York University, has proposed that Adam Smith’s Theory of Moral Sentiments (1759) has a more profound message for commerce and industry than The Wealth of Nations .

“Smith’s picture thus has a clear anti-Freudian thrust: it denies the hydraulic picture of human emotions according to which emotions build up “pressure” that must be “released.” Instead, and more plausibly, it conceives of emotions as things that can be controlled and trained by exercising what Smith calls “self-command.” The activity of reciprocal adjustment is then repeated numberless times in every person’s lifetime, as it is between and among the people in one’s community, resulting in the creation of an unintended and largely unconscious system of standards. These standards then become the rules by which we determine in any given case what kind of behavior is, as Smith calls it, “proper” in a situation and what “improper”—meaning what others can reasonably be expected to enter into.” Otteson (2000 , November 01).

Smith was a great admirer of the messages of music and wrote about the communication of its poetic massages in his essay Of the Nature of that Imitation which takes place in what are called the Imitative Arts , published in 1777 ( Smith, 1777/1982 ).

Beginning with his A Treatise of Human Nature (1739), David Hume strove to create a natural science of human psychology in opposition to René Descartes’ rationalism. He concluded that desire rather than reason motivates our behavior. Anticipating Merleau-Ponty’s phenomenology he also argued against the existence of innate ideas, concluding that we know only what we directly experience. He held that inductive reasoning and causality cannot be justified rationally, rather we follow custom and constant relations between ideas rather than logic. He concluded that we do not have a ‘conception of the self,’ only sensations of being alive. Following his teacher Hutcheson (1729) , he believed that ethics are based on feelings rather than abstract moral principles.

Finally, there is a bold clarity in the work of Thomas Reid, the third great follower of the teachings of Hutcheson, and a vigorous debating companion to David Hume. He wrote An Inquiry into the Human Mind on the Principles of Common Sense ( Reid, 1764 ).

Reid founded the Scottish School of Common Sense. For him ‘common sense’ is based on a direct experience of external reality, experience that becomes internal in language, which is based on an innate capacity pre-dating human consciousness, and acting as an instrument for that consciousness. He distinguished the acoustic element from the meanings which seem to have nothing to do with the sounds as such, a state of language, which he calls ‘artificial,’ that cannot be the primeval one, which he terms ‘natural.’ He described the way a child learns language by imitating sounds, becoming aware of them long before he or she understands the meaning in the artificial state of contemporary adult speech. If, says Reid, children were to understand immediately the conceptual content of the words they hear, they would never learn to speak at all. Here Reid distinguishes between natural and artificial signs.

‘It is by natural signs chiefly that we give force and energy to language; and the less language has of them, it is the less expressive and persuasive…. Artificial signs signify, but they do not express; they speak to the intellect, as algebraic characters may do, but the passions and the affections and the will hear them not: these continue dormant and inactive, till we speak to them in the language of nature , to which they are all attention and obedience.’ ( Reid, 1764 , p. 52).

‘Language of nature’ we equate with our embodied moving consciousness – our communicative musicality. An excess of ‘artificial signs,’ perhaps aimed at increasing productivity, leads to loneliness and ruthless rationality. However, the cultivation of our communicative musicality, in ourselves and others, through playful music, dance, ritual and sympathetic companionship, makes our communal life of shared work of the body and mind creative in more hopeful ways. It restores our common humanity and our connection with all living things.

Ethics Statement

Informed consent was gained for all data presented in this paper.

Author Contributions

SM contributed to all sections of the paper, particularly sections Communicative Musicality and Education into the Culture of Music and Communicative Musicality and Resilience of the Human Spirit. CT contributed to all sections of the paper, particularly sections The Genesis of Music in Infancy – A Short History of Discoveries, Case Studies of Infant Musicality, and Musical Affections of the Embodied Human Brain.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

  • ^ We regard the term ‘infant directed,’ usually used in this context, to be a misnomer that does not acknowledge the co-operative and shared nature of the interaction. We use ‘infant-inclusive’ to refer to the style of speech and singing that spontaneously occur between caregiver and infant.
  • ^ Written consent was obtained from the client by the first author.

Bannan, N., and Woodward, S. (2009). “Spontaneity in the musicality and music learning of children,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 465–494.

Google Scholar

Barrett, M. S. (2011). Musical narratives: a study of a young child’s identity work in and through music-making. Psychol. Music 39, 403–423. doi: 10.1177/0305735610373054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, M. C. (1971). The interpersonal context of infant vocalization. Quart. Progr. Rep. Res. Labor. Electron. 100, 170–176.

Bateson, M. C. (1979). “The epigenesis of conversational interaction: a personal account of research development,” in Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication , ed. M. Bullowa (London: Cambridge University Press),63–77.

Bateson, P., and Martin, P. (2013). Play, Playfulness, Creativity and Innovation , 1st Edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139057691

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. (1966). The active search for information: from reflexes to the model of the future. (In Russian) Republished in English by Feigenberg, I. M., & Meijer, O. G. (1999). The active search for information: from reflexes to the model of the future. Motor Control 3, 225–236.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. (1967). Coordination and Regulation of Movements. New York, NY: Pergamon.

Birdwhistell, R. (1970). Kinesics and Context. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.

Bjørkvold, J.-R. (1992). The Muse Within: Creativity and Communication, Song and Play From Childhood Through Maturity. New York, NY: HarperCollins.

Blacking, J. (1995). Music, Culture, and Experience: Selected Papers of John Blacking (Chicago Studies in Ethnomusicology). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. Int. J. Psychoanal. 39, 1–23.

Bowman, W. (2012). “Music’s place in education,” in The Oxford Handbook of Music Education , eds G. McPherson and G. Welch (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).

Brazelton, T. B. (1961). Psychophysiologic reactions in the neonate. I: the value of observations of the neonate. J. Pediatr. 58, 508–512. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(61)80184-4

Brazelton, T. B. (1973). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (Clinics in Developmental Medicine, 50.) London: Heinemann Medical Books.

Brazelton, T. B. (1979). “Evidence of communication during neonatal behavioural assessment,” in Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication , ed. M. Bullowa (London: Cambridge University Press), 79–88.

Brazelton, T. B., and Nugent, J. K. (1995). The Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. Cambridge: Mac Keith Press.

Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a Theory of Instruction. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. S. (1968). Processes of Cognitive Growth: Infancy. (Heinz Werner Lectures, 1968). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press with Barri Publishers.

Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s Talk. Learning to Use Language. New York, NY: Norton.

Bruner, J. S. (1996). The Culture of Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. S. (2003). Making Stories: Law, Literature, Life. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Buber, M. (1923/1970). I and Thou (transl. W. Kaufmann). Edinburgh: T. and T. Clark.

Bullowa, M., ed. (1979). Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication. London: Cambridge University Press.

Busnel, M. C., Granier-Deferre, C., and Lecanuet, J. P. (1992). Fetal audition. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 662, 118–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb22857.x

Buzsáki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301069.001.0001

Champagne, R. A. (1990). “The semiotics of lévi-strauss: translation as communication,” in The Semiotic Web, 1989 , eds T. A. Sebeok and J. Umiker-Sebeok (Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter), 61–88.

Chiron, C., Jambaque, I., Nabbout, R., Lounes, R., Syrota, A., and Dulac, O. (1997). The right brain hemisphere is dominant in human infants. Brain 120, 1057–1065. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.6.1057

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chukovsky, K. (1963). From Two to Five. (Trans K. M. Morton). Berkley, CA: University of California Press.

Custodero, L. A. (2009). “Intimacy and reciprocity in improvisatory musical performance: pedagogical lessons from adult artists and young children,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 513–530.

Damasio, A. R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. London: Heinemann.

Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain. San Diego, CA: Harcourt.

Damasio, A. R. (2010). Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain. New York City, NY: Pantheon.

de Vries, J. I. P., Visser, G. H. A., and Prechtl, H. F. R. (1984). “Fetal motility in the first half of pregnancy,” in Continuity of Neural Functions from Prenatal to Postnatal Life , ed. H. F. R. Prechtl (Oxford: Blackwell), 46–64.

Delamont, R. S., Julu, P. O. O., and Jamal, G. A. (1999). Periodicity of a noninvasive measure of cardiac vagal tone during non-rapid eye movement sleep in non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived normal subjects. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 16, 146–153. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199903000-00007

Dewey, J. (1938). Experience and Education. New York, NY: Macmillan Co.

Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Donaldson, M. (1992). Human Minds: An Exploration. London: Allen Lane/Penguin Books.

Eckerdal, P., and Merker, B. (2009). “‘Music’ and the ‘action song’ in infant development: an interpretation,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 241–262.

Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989). Human Ethology. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

Eisenberg, N., and Sulik, M. J. (2012). Is Self–Other overlap the key to understanding empathy? Emot. Rev. 4, 34–35.

Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. (1975). Unmasking the Face: A Guide to Recognizing Emotions From Facial Clues. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Flohr, J., and Trevarthen, C. (2008), “Music learning in childhood: Early developments of a musical brain and body,” in Neurosciences In Music Pedagogy , eds W. Gruhn and F. Rauscher (New York, NY: Nova Biomedical Books), 53–100.

Frank, B., and Trevarthen, C. (2012). “Intuitive meaning: supporting impulses for interpersonal life in the sociosphere of human knowledge, practice, and language,” in Moving Ourselves, Moving Others: Motion and Emotion in Intersubjectivity, Consciousness, and Language , eds A. Foolen, U. Lüdtke, T. P. Racine, and J. Zlatev (Philadelphia: John Benjamins), 261–303. doi: 10.1075/ceb.6.11fra

Freud, S. (1923). Das Ich und das Es (The Ego and the Id). Vienna: Internationaler Psycho-Analytischer Verlag.

Fröhlich, C. (2009). “Vitality in music and dance as basic existential experience: applications in teaching music,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 495–512.

Gabrielsson, A., and Juslin, P. N. (1996). Emotional expression in music performance: between the performer’s intention and the listener’s experience. Psychol. Music 24, 68–91. doi: 10.1177/0305735696241007

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Goodrich, B. G. (2010). We do, therefore we think: time, motility, and consciousness. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 331–361. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2010.21.5.331

Gratier, M., and Apter-Danon, G. (2009). “The improvised musicality of belonging: repetition and variation in mother–infant vocal interaction,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 301–327.

Gratier, M., and Trevarthen, C. (2008). Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother-infant vocal interaction. J. Conscious. Stud. 15, 122–158.

Hadamard, J. (1945). The Psychology of Invention in the Mathematical Field. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Hay, D., and Nye, R. (1998). The Spirit of the Child. London: Fount (Harper-Collins).

Hepper, P. G. (1991). An examination of fetal learning before and after birth. Irish J. Psychol. 12, 95–107. doi: 10.1080/03033910.1991.10557830

Hess, W. R. (1954). Diencephalon: Autonomic and Extrapyramidal Functions. Orlando, FL: Grune and Stratton.

Hobson, R. F. (1985). Forms of Feeling. London: Tavistock.

Hume, D. (1739–1740). A Treatise on Human Nature: Being an Attempt to Introduce the Experimental Method of Reasoning Into Moral Subjects. London: The Floating Press.

Hutcheson, F. (1729). An Inquiry into the Original of Our Ideas of Beauty and Virtue: In Two Treatises Concerning Beauty, Order, Harmony, Design. 2. Concerning Moral Good and Evil. London: J. and J. Knapton.

Hutcheson, F. (1755). “A system of moral philosophy,” in Three Books , ed. R. Glasgow (London: A. Millar, and T. Longman. (published posthumously by his son)).

Imberty, M. (1981). Les Ecriture du Temps. Paris: Dunod.

Imberty, M. (2000). “The question of innate competencies in musical communication,” in The Origins of Music , eds N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 449–462.

Ingold, T. (2018). Anthropology And/as Education. New York, NY: Routledge.

James, W. (1892/1985). Psychology: The Briefer Course , ed. G. Allport. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

Juslin, P. N. (1997). Can results from studies of perceived expression in musical performances be generalized across response formats? Psychomusicology 16, 77–101. doi: 10.1037/h0094065

Juslin, P. N. (2001). “Communicating emotion in music performance: a review and theoretical framework,” in Music and Emotion: Theory and Research , eds P. N. Juslin and J. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 309–337.

Kirschner, S., and Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 354–364. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004

Kugiumutzakis, G. (1993). “Intersubjective vocal imitation in early mother-infant interaction,” in New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development , eds J. Nadel and L. Camaioni (London: Routledge), 23–47.

Kugiumutzakis, G., and Trevarthen, C. (2015). “Neonatal Imitation,” in International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences , 2nd Edn, Vol. 16, eds D. James and Wright (Oxford: Elsevier), 481–488. ISBN: 9780080970868. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23160-7

Kurth, E. (1991). Selected Writings (eds L. A. Rothfarb, Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kühl, O. (2007). Musical semantics. European Semiotics: Language, Cognition and Culture, No. 7. Bern: Peter Lang. doi: 10.3726/978-3-0351-0271-0

CrossRef Full Text

Langer, S. K. (1942). Philosophy in a New Key: A Study in the Symbolism of Reason, Rite, and Art. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Lashley, K. S. (1951). “The problem of serial order in behavior,” in Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior , ed. L. A. Jeffress (New York, NY: Wiley), 112–136.

Llinás, R. (2001). I of the Vortex: From Neurons to Self. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

MacDonald, R. A. R., Hargreaves, D. J., and Miell, D., eds (2002). Musical Identities. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

MacDonald, R. A. R and Miell, D. (2004). “Musical collaboration,” in Learning To Collaborate, Collaborating To Learn: Understanding and Promoting Educationally Productive Collaborative Work , eds K. Littleton and D. Miell (New York, NY: Nova Science), 133–146.

Malloch, S. (1999). “Mother and Infants and Communicative Musicality,” in Rhythms, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human Communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999-2000 , ed. I. Deliège (Liège: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music), 29–57.

Malloch, S. (2017). “Establishing a therapy of musicality: The embodied narratives of myself with others,” in Companionship: The Rhythm of Relating in Children’s Therapies , eds S. Daniel and C. Trevarthen (London: Jessica Kingsley),63–81.

Malloch, S., Sharp, D., Campbell, D. M., Campbell, A. M., and Trevarthen, C. (1997). Measuring the human voice: analysing pitch, timing, loudness and voice quality in mother/infant communication. Proc. Inst. Acoust. 19, 495–500.

Malloch, S., Shoemark, H., Črnèec, R., Newnham, C., Paul, C., Prior, M., et al. (2012). Music therapy with hospitalized infants—the art and science of communicative musicality. Infant Ment. Health J. 33, 386–399. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21346

Malloch, S., and Trevarthen, C., eds (2009a). Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Malloch, S., and Trevarthen, C. (2009b). “Musicality: communicating the vitality and interests of life,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 1–11.

Maratos, O. (1973) The Origin and Development of Imitation in the First Six Months of Life. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Geneva, Geneva.

Maratos, O. (1982). “Trends in the development of imitation in the firsts six months of life,” in Regressions in Mental Development: Basic Phenomena and Theories , ed. T. G. Bever (Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum), 81–101.

Maratos, O. (1998). “Psychoanalysis and the management of pervasive developmental disorders, including autism,” in eds Children With Autism: Diagnosis and Interventions To Meet Their Needs , 2nd Edn, eds C. Trevarthen, K. J. Aitken, D. Papoudi, J. Z. Robarts (London: Jessica Kingsley),203–214.

Maturana, H., Mpodozis, J., and Carlos Letelier, J. (1995). Brain, language and the origin of human mental functions. Biol. Res. 28, 15–26.

Maturana, H., and Verden-Zoller, G. (2008). Origin of Humanness in the Biology of Love. Edited by Pille Bullen. Charlottesville, VA: Imprint Academic.

Maturana, H. R., and Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoesis and Cognition. Dordrecht: D. Reidel. doi: 10.1007/978-94-009-8947-4

Mazokopaki, M., and Kugiumutzakis, G. (2009). “Infant rhythms: expressions of musical companionship,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, 185-208 , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

McGilchrist, I. (2009). The Master and His Emissary: The Divided Brain and the Making of the Western World. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. ISBN 0-300-14878-X.

Meares, R. (2005). The Metaphor of Play: Origin and Breakdown of Personal Being. New York, NY: Routledge.

Meares, R. (2016). The Poet’s Voice in the Making of Mind. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9781315670638

Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198, 75–78. doi: 10.1126/science.198.4312.75

Merker, B. (2009). “Ritual foundations of human uniqueness,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 45–60.

Merleau-Ponty, M. (2012 [1945]). Phenomenology of Perception , trans. D. A. Landes. New York: Routledge.

Mithen, S. (2005). The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.

Moog, H. (1976). The Musical Experience of the Pre-School Child. London: Schott Music.

Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C. (1985). “Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers,” in Social Perception in Infants , eds T. M. Field and N. A. Fox (Norwood, NJ: Ablex), 177–197.

Nagy, E. (2011). The newborn infant: a missing stage in developmental psychology. Infant Child Dev. 20, 3–19. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.12.007

Nagy, E., and Molnár, P. (1994). Homo imitans or homo provocans? Abstract, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 18:128. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(94)90394-8

Nagy, E., and Molnár, P. (2004). Homo imitans or homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behav. Dev. 27, 54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.06.004

Narvaez, D. (2014). Neurobiology and the Development of Human Morality: Evolution, Culture and Wisdom. New York: W. W. Norton.

Ockelford, A. (2017). Comparing Notes: How We Make Sense of Music. London: Profile Books.

Osborne, N. (2009a). “Towards a chronobiology of musical rhythm,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press),545–564.

Osborne, N. (2009b). “Music for children in zones of conflict and post-conflict: a psychobiological approach,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 331–356.

Osborne, N. (2017). “Love, rhythm and chronobiology,” in Rhythms of Relating in Children’s Therapies: Connecting Creatively with Vulnerable Children , eds S. Daniel and C. Trevarthen (London: Jessica Kingsley), 14–27.

Otteson, J. R. (2000). Adam Smith: Moral Philosopher. Available at: https://fee.org/articles/adam-smith-moral-philosopher/

Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience.: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Panksepp, J., Asma, S., Curran, G., Gabriel, R., and Greif, T. (2012). The philosophical implications of affective neuroscience. J. Consciousn. Stud. 19, 6–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.010

Panksepp, J., and Bernatzky, G. (2002). Emotional sounds and the brain: the neuro-affective foundations of musical appreciation. Behav. Process. 60, 133–155. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00080-3

Panksepp, J., and Biven, L. (2012). Archaeology of Mind: Neuroevolutionary Origins of Human Emotions. New York, NY: Norton.

Panksepp, J., and Trevarthen, C. (2009). “The neuroscience of emotion in music,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 105–146.

Papoušek, H. (1996). “Musicality in infancy research: biological and cultural origins of early musicality,” in Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of Musical Competence , eds I. Deliège and J. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford university press), 37–55. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523321.003.0002

Pavlicevic, M. (1997). Music Therapy in Context: Music Meaning and Relationship . London: Jessica Kingsley. 4341.

Pavlicevic, M. (1999). Music Therapy: Intimate Notes. London: Jessica Kingsley.

Pavlicevic, M. (2000). Improvisation in music therapy: human communication in sound. J. Music Ther. 37, 269–285. doi: 10.1093/jmt/37.4.269

Pavlicevic, M., and Ansdell, G. (2009). “Between communicative musicality and collaborative musicing: a perspective from community music therapy,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 357–376.

Piaget, J. (1958). The Child’s Construction of Reality. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Piaget, J. (1966). The Psychology of Intelligence. Totawa, NJ: Littlefield Adams.

Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: W. W. Norton.

Porges, S. W. (2011). The Polyvagal Theory: Neurophysiological Foundations of Emotions, Attachment, Communication, and Self-regulation. New York, NY: WW Norton.

Reddy, V. (2008). How Infants Know Minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Reid, T. (1764). An Inquiry into the Human Mind on the Principles of Common Sense. London: A. Millar, and Edinburgh: A. Kincaid and J. Bell. (Republished 1997, Edinburgh University Press). doi: 10.1037/11974-000

Rodrigues, H. M., Rodrigues, P. M., and Correia, J. S. (2009). “Communicative Musicality as creative participation: from early childhood to advanced performance,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship , eds S. Malloch and C. Trevarthen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 585–610.

Rogoff, B. (2003). The Cultural Nature of Human Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sachs, C. (1943). The Rise of Music in the Ancient World, East and West. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.

Sacks, O. (2006). The power of music. Brain 129, 2528–2532. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl234

Sander, L. W. (1964). Adaptive relationships in early mother-child interaction. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 3, 231–264. doi: 10.1016/S0002-7138(09)61920-8

Sander, L. W. (1975). “Infant and caretaking environment: investigation and conceptualization of adaptive behavior in a system of increasing complexity,” in Explorations in Child Psychiatry , ed. E. James Anthony. (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 129–166.

Sander, L. W. (2008). Living Systems, Evolving Consciousness, and the Emerging Person. (eds G. Amadei and I. Bianchi; Psychoanalytic Inquiry), Vol. 26. New York, NY: Analytic Press.

Scheflen, A. E. (1972). The Stream and Structure of Communicational Behavior. Bloomington: University of Indiana Press.

Schögler, B., and Trevarthen, C. (2007). “To sing and dance together: from infants to jazz,” in On Being Moved: From Mirror Neurons to Empathy , ed. S. Bråten (Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins), 281–302. doi: 10.1075/aicr.68.22sch

Sherrington, C. S. (1955). Man On His Nature. London: Penguin Books Ltd.

Small, C. (1998). Musicking: The Meanings of Performing and Listening. Hanover, NH: Wesleyan University Press.

Smith, A. (1759). Theory of Moral Sentiments. Edinburgh (Modern Edition: D. D. Raphael and A.L. Macfie, General Editors, Glasgow Edition). Oxford: Clarendon. doi: 10.1093/oseo/instance.00042831

Smith, A. (1777/1982). “Of the nature of that Imitation which takes place in what are called the imitative arts,” in The Glasgow Edition of the Works and Correspondence of Adam Smith, Essays on Philosophical Subjects , Vol. 3, eds W. P. D. Wightman and J. C. Bryce (Indianapolis, IN: Liberty Fund).

Solms, M., and Panksepp, J. (2012). The “Id” knows more than the “Ego” admits: Neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience. Brain Sci. 2, 147–175. doi: 10.3390/brainsci2020147

Spitz, R. A. (1945). Hospitalism—an inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanal. Study Child 1, 53–74. doi: 10.1080/00797308.1945.11823126

Spitz, R. A. (1957). No and Yes: On the Genesis of Human Communication. New York, NY: International Universities Press.

Spitz, R. A., and Wolf, K. M. (1946). The smiling response: a contribution to the orthogenesis of social relations. Genet. Psychol. Monogr. 34, 57–125.

Stern, D., Sander, L., Hahum, J., Harrison, A., Lyons-Ruth, K., Morgan, A., et al. (1998). Non-interpretive mechanisms in psychoanalytic therapy: the ‘something more’ than interpretation. Int. J. Psychoanal. 79:903.

Stern, D. N. (1971). A micro-analysis of mother-infant interaction: behaviors regulating social contact between a mother and her three-and-a-half-month-old twins. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 10, 501–517. doi: 10.1016/S0002-7138(09)61752-0

Stern, D. N. (1985). The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Development Psychology. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Stern, D. N. (1995). The Motherhood Constellation. New York: Basic Books.

Stern, D. N. (2000). The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Development Psychology , 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Stern, D. N. (2004). The Present Moment: In Psychotherapy and Everyday Life. New York, NY: Norton.

Stern, D. N. (2010). Forms of Vitality: Exploring Dynamic Experience in Psychology, the Arts, Psychotherapy and Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/med:psych/9780199586066.001.0001

Stige, B. (2002). Culture-Centered Music Therapy , Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers.

Tafuri, J., and Hawkins, E. (2008). Infant Musicality: New Research for Educators and Parents. Aldershot, Hants, England. Burlington, VT: Ashgate.

Thatcher, R. W., Walker, R. A., and Giudice, S. (1987). Human cerebral hemispheres develop at different rates and ages. Science 236, 1110–1113. doi: 10.1126/science.3576224

Trainor, L. J., and Cirelli, L. (2015). Rhythm and interpersonal synchrony in early social development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1337, 45–52. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12649

Trehub, S. E. (2003). The developmental origins of musicality. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 669–673. doi: 10.1038/nn1084

Trehub, S. E., and Hannon, E. E. (2006). Infant music perception: domain-general or domain-specific mechanisms? Cognition 100, 73–99.

Trehub, S. E., and Trainor, L. J. (1998). Singing to infants: lullabies and playsongs. Adv. Infancy Res. 12, 43–77.

Trevarthen, C. (1974). The psychobiology of speech development. In, E.H. Lenneberg (Ed.), Language and Brain: Developmental Aspects. Neurosci. Res. Program Bull. 12, 570-585.

Trevarthen, C. (1977). “Descriptive analyses of infant communication behaviour,” in Studies in Mother-Infant Interaction: The Loch Lomond Symposium , ed. H. R. Schaffer (London: Academic Press), 227–270.

Trevarthen, C. (1980). “Neurological development and the growth of psychological functions,” in Developmental Psychology and Society , ed. J. Sants (London: Macmillans), 46-95. doi: 10.1007/978-1-349-16331-1_3

Trevarthen, C. (1984a). “Emotions in infancy: regulators of contacts and relationships with persons,” in Approaches to Emotion , eds K. Scherer and P. Ekman (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 129–157.

Trevarthen, C. (1984b). “How control of movements develops,” in Human Motor Actions: Bernstein Reassessed. Amsterdam , ed. H. T. A. Whiting (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 223–261. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61374-6

Trevarthen, C. (1986). “Development of intersubjective motor control in infants,” in Motor Development in Children: Aspects of Coordination and Control , eds M. G. Wade and H. T. A. Whiting (Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhof),209–261.

Trevarthen, C. (1989). “Motives for culture in young children – their natural development through communication,” in The Nature of Culture , ed. W. Koch (Bochum: Brockmeyer), 80-119.

Trevarthen, C. (1990a). “Signs before speech,” in The Semiotic Web, 1989 , eds T. A. Sebeok and J. Umiker-Sebeok (Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter), 689–755.

Trevarthen, C. (1990b). “Growth and education of the hemispheres,” in Brain Circuits and Functions of the Mind: Essays in Honour of Roger W. Sperry , ed. C. Trevarthen (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 334–363.

Trevarthen, C. (1999). Musicality and the Intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Sci. Fall 3 (Suppl.), 155–211. doi: 10.1177/10298649000030S109

Trevarthen, C. (2001). Intrinsic motives for companionship in understanding: their origin, development and significance for infant mental health. Infant Mental Health J. 22, 95–131. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(200101/04)22:1<95::AID-IMHJ4>3.0.CO;2-6

Trevarthen, C. (2004). “Brain development,” in Oxford Companion to the Mind , 2nd Edn, ed. R. L. Gregory (Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press), 116–127.

Trevarthen, C. (2005a). First things first: infants make good use of the sympathetic rhythm of imitation, without reason or language. J. Child Psychother. 31, 91–113. doi: 10.1080/00754170500079651

Trevarthen, C. (2005b). “Stepping away from the mirror: pride and shame in adventures of companionship Reflections on the nature and emotional needs of infant intersubjectivity,” in Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis. Dahlem Workshop Report 92 , eds C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossman, S. B. Hrdy, M. E. Lamb, S. W. Porges, et al. (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press),55–84.

Trevarthen, C. (2008). The musical art of infant conversation: narrating in the time of sympathetic experience, without rational interpretation, before words. Music. Sci. 12(Suppl.), 15–46. doi: 10.1177/1029864908012001021

Trevarthen, C. (2009). “Human biochronology: on the source and functions of ‘musicality’,” in Music That Works: Contributions of Biology, Neurophysiology, Psychology, Sociology, Medicine and Musicology. eds R. Haas and V. Brandes (New York, NY: Springer), 221–265.

Trevarthen, C. (2015). Infant semiosis: the psycho-biology of action and shared experience from birth. Cognit. Dev. 36, 130–141. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.09.008

Trevarthen, C. (2016). The spiritual nature of the infant self: an imaginative actor in relations of affection. J. Consciousn. Stud. 23, 258–282.

Trevarthen, C., and Bjørkvold, J.-R. (2016). “Life for learning: how a young child seeks joy with companions in a meaningful world,” in Contexts for Young Child Flourishing: Evolution, Family and Society , eds D. Narvaez, J. Braungart-Rieker, L. Miller-Graff, L. Gettler, and P. Hastings (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 28–60. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190237790.003.0002

Trevarthen, C., Delafield-Butt, J., and Dunlop, A.-W. (2018). The Child’s Curriculum: Understanding the Natural Talents of Young Children, Allowing Rich Holistic Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Trevarthen, C., and Malloch, S. (2000). The dance of wellbeing: defining the musical therapeutic effect. Norwegian J. Music Ther. 9, 3–17. doi: 10.1080/08098130009477996

Trevarthen, C., and Malloch, S. (2017a). “The musical self: affections for life in a community of sound,” in The Oxford Handbook of Musical Identities , 2nd Edn, Chap. 9, eds R. MacDonald, D. Hargreaves, D. Miell (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 155–175. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199679485.003.0009

Trevarthen, C., and Malloch, S. (2017b). “Grace in moving and joy in sharing: the intrinsic beauty of communicative musicality from birth,” in Anthropology and Beauty: From Aesthetics to Creativity , Chap. 8, ed. S. Bunn (London: Taylor & Francis/Routledge), 115–130.

Turner, V. (1982). From Ritual to Theatre: The Human Seriousness of Play. New York, NY: Performing Arts Journal Publications.

Turner, V. (1983). “Play and drama: the horns of a dilemma,” in The World of Play. Proceedings of the 7th Annual Meeting of the Association of the Anthropological Study of Play , ed. F. E. Manning (West Point, NY: Leisure Press), 217–224.

Valiente, C., Swanson, J., and Eisenberg, N. (2012). Linking students’ emotions and academic achievement: when and why emotions matter. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 129–135. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00192.x

van Rees, S., and de Leeuw, R. (1993). Born too early: the kangaroo method with premature babies, video by stichting lichaamstaal. Scheyvenhofweg 12:6093.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1966). “Genesis of the higher mental functions. Abridged translation,” in Learning to Think , eds P. H. Light, S. Sheldon, and M. Woodhead (London: Routledge and Open University Press), 32–41.

Whitehead, A. N. (1929). The Aims of Education and Other Essays. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Whiten, A. (2000). “The evolution of deep social mind in humans,” in The Descent of Mind , eds M. Corballis and S. Lea (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Zeedyk, M. S. (2006). From intersubjectivity to subjectivity: the transformative roles of emotional intimacy and imitation. Infant Child Dev. 15, 321–344. doi: 10.1002/icd.457

Zentner, M., Eerola, T., and Purves, D. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 5768–5773. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000121107

Keywords : musicking, motor intelligence, gestural narrative, infant musicality, cultural learning

Citation: Malloch S and Trevarthen C (2018) The Human Nature of Music. Front. Psychol. 9:1680. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01680

Received: 18 February 2018; Accepted: 21 August 2018; Published: 04 October 2018.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2018 Malloch and Trevarthen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephen Malloch, [email protected] Colwyn Trevarthen, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Search form

How to Live Better, Longer

ultra processed food

Ultra Processed Food Linked To Early Death, Study Identifies Items With Highest Risk

olive oil

Olive Oil In Diet Reduces Risk Of Death From Dementia: Study

exercise

Healthy Lifestyle At 40 Adds 5 Years To Life, Offsets Genetic Risk: Study

smoking

Smokers Who Quit Before 40 Have Similar Life-Expectancy Of Non-smokers: Study

postpartum depression

Maternal Mental Health Month: Expert Shares Tips To Support Someone Going Through Postpartum Depression

bird watching

A Simple Nature-Based Activity Can Improve Students' Mental Health, Says Study

depression

Women Are 40% More At Risk Of Depression During Perimenopause: Study

seafood

An Omega-6 Fatty Acid Linked To Reduced Risk Of Bipolar Disorder: Study

Karen Corona

Explore The Healing Power Of Expressive Arts With Wellness Coach Karen Corona

Dr Jason

Dr. Jason Shumard Revolutionizes Holistic Healing And Transformative Wellness

Thermal Earring: Low-power Wireless Earring for Longitudinal Earlobe Temperature Sensing

Thermal Earring To Monitor Temperature: Experts Say It Could Also Track Ovulation And Stress

pregnancy test

First Saliva-Based Pregnancy Tests: Everything To Know

aging

Discrimination Linked To Faster Aging; White People Experience More Impact: Study

night shift workers

Study Explains How Night Shift Workers Are Prone To Diabetes, Obesity

sedentary lifestyle in childhood

Sedentary Lifestyle In Childhood Linked To Progressing Heart Enlargement, Light Activity Reverses Risk: Study

grains

Ancient Grains Help Improve Blood Sugar, Cholesterol Levels In Diabetes Patients: Study

metformin

Metformin Reduces COVID-19 Hospitalizations, Long COVID: Study

asthma

No Link Between COVID-19 Virus And Asthma Risk In Kids, Says Study

fatigue

Study Says 'Double Jointed' People At Higher Risk Of Long COVID

COVID-19

COVID-19 Virus Lingers In The Body More Than A Year After Infection: Studies

It's a small world: music can cross cultural barriers and induce same emotions in people, despite background.

psy

  • Share on Twitter
  • Share on Facebook
  • Share on Pocket

The catchy Korean pop song " Gangnam Style " broke records in the summer of 2012 when so many people watched the song on YouTube that, for the first time ever, the site had to upgrade its viewing capacity. The song became popular throughout the world despite being sung entirely in Korean, a language most are unfamiliar with outside of Asia. A recent study , however, may have provided a bit of insight into why the regional song gained international success. The study found that music was able to transcend all cultural and lingual barriers and elicit the same emotions in individuals, regardless of their background.

What do hipsters living in Montreal and tribesmen in the Congo have in common? Apparently, their taste for music. Researchers from both Canada and Germany travelled deep into the African bush to play Western music to a tribe of Pygmy. The 11 musical clips were around 20 to 90 seconds long in length. Back in North America, the researchers then played eight Pygmy songs to Canadian “hipsters.” Both the Canadians and Pygmies were chosen for the study based on either their amateur or professional musical abilities.

For the African listeners, researchers chose songs from popular Western films such as Star Wars and Schindler’s List that induced an array of emotions, from calmness to sadness. What they chose for the Canadians, on the other hand, was music Pygmies associated with emotional cultural events, such as funeral ceremony songs and childhood lullabies.

To overcome the language barrier, volunteers chose an emoticon that most closely communicated the feelings the music elicited in them. Researchers also recorded various body measurements such as heart rate and breathing. In the end, results showed that both groups experienced similar responses to the music, both in the emoticons they chose and the way their bodies reacted.

"This is probably due to certain low-level aspects of music such as tempo (or beat), pitch (how high or low the music is on the scale), and timbre (tone color or quality), but this will need further research," said Dr. Hauke Egermann, a researcher who took part in the study, in a press release. The only significant difference was that the Canadians described a much wider array of emotions, but the study authors wrote this difference is probably caused by the varying role that music plays in both cultures.

"First of all, the results of this study help to understand in general ... why music is emotional for us," Egermann said in an email to Medical Daily . "Thus this research we will be able to understand the different factors and mechanisms that are at play when music impacts on us. Secondly, these findings could, of course, also be used whenever music is used in a functional way for communication, therapy, marketing, etc.'

Source: Egermann H, Fernando N, Chuen L, McAdams S. Music induces universal emotion-related psychophysiological responses: comparing Canadian listeners to Congolese Pygmies. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015.

Allergies

  • Alzheimer's
  • Amputation/Prosthetics

Dental

  • Dengue Fever
  • Dental Health
  • Dermatological Disorders
  • Developmental Disorders
  • Digestive Disorders
  • Down Syndrome

Gerontology

  • Gastrointestinal Disorders
  • Genetic Disorders
  • Genital Warts
  • Geriatric care
  • Gerontology
  • Gum Disease
  • Gynecological Disorders
  • Head And Neck Cancer

Liver Disease

  • Kidney Cancer
  • Kidney Disease
  • Knee Problems
  • Lead Poisoning
  • Liver Disease
  • Low Testosterone
  • Lung Cancer

Mental health

  • Macular Degeneration
  • Men's Health
  • Menstruation/Periods
  • Mental Health
  • Metabolic Disorders

Pain

  • Pancreatic Cancer
  • Parasitic Infections
  • Parkinson's Disease
  • Pediatric Diseases

Sleep

  • Schizophrenia
  • Senior Health
  • Sexual Health
  • Sickle Cell Disease
  • Skin Cancer
  • Sleep Apnea

Women health

  • Uterine Cancer
  • Varicose Veins
  • Viral Infection
  • Women's Health
  • Yeast Infection

Essay on Music for Students and Children

500+ words essay on music.

Music is a vital part of different moments of human life. It spreads happiness and joy in a person’s life. Music is the soul of life and gives immense peace to us. In the words of William Shakespeare, “If music is the food of love, play on, Give me excess of it; that surfeiting, The appetite may sicken, and so die.” Thus, Music helps us in connecting with our souls or real self.

Essay on Music

What is Music?

Music is a pleasant sound which is a combination of melodies and harmony and which soothes you. Music may also refer to the art of composing such pleasant sounds with the help of the various musical instruments. A person who knows music is a Musician.

The music consists of Sargam, Ragas, Taals, etc. Music is not only what is composed of men but also which exists in nature. Have you ever heard the sound of a waterfall or a flowing river ? Could you hear music there? Thus, everything in harmony has music. Here, I would like to quote a line by Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart, one of the greatest musicians, “The music is not in the notes, but in the silence between.”

Importance of Music:

Music has great qualities of healing a person emotionally and mentally. Music is a form of meditation. While composing or listening music ones tends to forget all his worries, sorrows and pains. But, in order to appreciate good music, we need to cultivate our musical taste. It can be cited that in the Dwapar Yug, the Gopis would get mesmerized with the music that flowed from Lord Krishna’s flute. They would surrender themselves to Him. Also, the research has proved that the plants which hear the Music grow at a faster rate in comparison to the others.

Get the huge list of more than 500 Essay Topics and Ideas

Magical Powers of Music:

It has the power to cure diseases such as anxiety, depression, insomnia, etc. The power of Music can be testified by the legends about Tansen of his bringing the rains by singing Raag Megh Malhar and lighting lamps by Raga Deepak. It also helps in improving the concentration and is thus of great help to the students.

Conclusion:

Music is the essence of life. Everything that has rhythm has music. Our breathing also has a rhythm. Thus, we can say that there is music in every human being or a living creature. Music has the ability to convey all sorts of emotions to people. Music is also a very powerful means to connect with God. We can conclude that Music is the purest form of worship of God and to connect with our soul.

FAQs on Essay on Music:

Q.1. Why is Music known as the Universal Language?

Ans.1. Music is known as the Universal language because it knows no boundaries. It flows freely beyond the barriers of language, religion, country, etc. Anybody can enjoy music irrespective of his age.

Q.2. What are the various styles of Music in India?

Ans.2. India is a country of diversities. Thus, it has numerous styles of music. Some of them are Classical, Pop, Ghazals, Bhajans, Carnatic, Folk, Khyal, Thumri, Qawwali, Bhangra, Drupad, Dadra, Dhamar, Bandish, Baithak Gana, Sufi, Indo Jazz, Odissi, Tarana, Sugama Sangeet, Bhavageet, etc.

Customize your course in 30 seconds

Which class are you in.

tutor

  • Travelling Essay
  • Picnic Essay
  • Our Country Essay
  • My Parents Essay
  • Essay on Favourite Personality
  • Essay on Memorable Day of My Life
  • Essay on Knowledge is Power
  • Essay on Gurpurab
  • Essay on My Favourite Season
  • Essay on Types of Sports

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Download the App

Google Play

IELTS Mentor "IELTS Preparation & Sample Answer"

  • Skip to content
  • Jump to main navigation and login

Nav view search

  • IELTS Sample

IELTS Writing Task 2/ Essay Topics with sample answer.

Ielts essay # 1219 - music for bringing people of different cultures and ages together, ielts writing task 2/ ielts essay:, some people say that music is a good way of bringing people of different cultures and ages together., to what extent do you agree or disagree with this opinion.

  • IELTS Essay
  • Writing Task 2
  • Agree/Disagree Essay

music has no boundaries essay

IELTS Materials

  • IELTS Bar Graph
  • IELTS Line Graph
  • IELTS Table Chart
  • IELTS Flow Chart
  • IELTS Pie Chart
  • IELTS Letter Writing
  • Academic Reading

Useful Links

  • IELTS Secrets
  • Band Score Calculator
  • Exam Specific Tips
  • Useful Websites
  • IELTS Preparation Tips
  • Academic Reading Tips
  • Academic Writing Tips
  • GT Writing Tips
  • Listening Tips
  • Speaking Tips
  • IELTS Grammar Review
  • IELTS Vocabulary
  • IELTS Cue Cards
  • IELTS Life Skills
  • Letter Types

IELTS Mentor - Follow Twitter

  • Privacy Policy
  • Cookie Policy
  • Copyright Notice
  • HTML Sitemap
  • Practice Test
  • Useful Tips – Tricks
  • Full Writing Review
  • General Writing Task
  • Writing Task 1
  • Writing Task 2
  • Writing Exercises
  • Writing Sample – Topics
  • Writing Vocabulary
  • Speaking Vocabulary
  • Intro Question
  • Speaking Part 1
  • Speaking Part 2
  • Speaking Part 2 – Audio
  • Speaking Part 3
  • IELTS Books
  • Recent Exams
  • IELTS Vocabulary
  • Essay from Examiners
  • IELTS Ideas

Logo

IELTS App - For Mobile

Ready for the IELTS exam with our IELTS app. Over 2 million downloads

Download App

Popular Last 24h

Writing task 2: any country should be able to sell goods to other countries without the restrictions of the government., ielts speaking part 2: describe coronavirus (covid-19), list of top 100 synonyms in the ielts test, ielts speaking part 3: bicycle, two-way discussion : education, describe a long bike, motorbike, or car trip that you would like to take, describe a person whom you met for the first time and made you happy.

  • IELTS Test/Skills FAQs
  • IELTS Scoring in Detail
  • Forecast Speaking – 2023
  • List IELTS Speaking Part 3
  • List IELTS Speaking Part 1
  • IELTS Writing 2023 – Actual Test

Our Telegram

Join our community for IELTS preparation and share and download materials.

The information on this site is for informational purposes only. IELTS is a registered trademark of the University of Cambridge ESOL, the British Council, and IDP Education Australia. This site and its owners are not affiliated, approved or endorsed by University of Cambridge ESOL, the British Council, or IDP Education Australia.

Latest Articles

Cue card – describe a gift you bought for someone, cue card – describe a place where you like to go shopping, ielts writing task 1 (process wasted glass bottles) – band 9, ielts speaking part 1: rubbish/ plastic garbage, talk about global warming (part 1/3), most popular, describe a film that made you laugh, topic: experience is the best teacher, describe something difficult you would like to succeed in doing, in many countries,today there are many highly qualified graduates without employment..

ieltspracticeonline All Rights Reserved

A business journal from the Wharton School of the University of Pennsylvania

Has Music Become Less Misogynistic?

May 14, 2024 • 5 min read.

If music reflects culture, what do misogynistic lyrics say about society? Wharton’s Jonah Berger uses AI to determine whether modern music has become less biased toward women over time.

Colorful illustration of a woman in profile wearing headphones and listening to music

  • Diversity, Equity, & Inclusion

When 19th-century poet Henry Wadsworth Longfellow wrote that music was “the universal language,” he hadn’t heard “Blurred Lines,” a song so misogynistic that it’s still controversial more than a decade after it climbed to the top of the Billboard charts.

Music has always been an expression of culture. But unlike the classical tunes of Longfellow’s day, modern music often has been criticized for lyrics that are sexist, derisive, and demeaning to women. Whether rock, pop, rap, or country, few genres have spared women from being the object of aggression.

In his study, “ Quantifying Cultural Change: Gender Bias in Music ,” Wharton marketing professor Jonah Berger used machine learning to answer the question: Has modern music become less misogynistic over time?

Berger and co-author Reihane Boghrati , information systems professor at Arizona State University’s W.P. Carey School of Business, analyzed more than 250,000 songs spanning 50 years and six genres — pop, rock, country, rap, dance, and R&B. They found improvement up until the mid-1990s, and then a leveling off or reversal. Male artists were mostly responsible for the downshift, because female artists were less biased to begin with.

“Culture is a self-reinforcing system.” — Jonah Berger

“While things have certainly improved over the last 50 years, they haven’t become neutral,” Berger said. “Lyrics are still biased against women.”

It’s hard to pinpoint a single reason for the decline in bias, although Berger attributes it in part to changing cultural values and perceptions. Musicians today risk backlash for songs filled with derogatory lyrics, no matter how catchy. Pharrell Williams , who co-wrote and sang on “Blurred Lines,” later said the controversy educated him about chauvinism and that he “didn’t realize that some of my songs catered to that.”

Music as a Measure of Culture

Berger, who’s an expert on social influence and how language shapes behavior, said music is an important measure of culture because of its prevalence. It’s a shared experience that influences the audience, much like popular books and movies.

“People sing along to lyrics without thinking about what they are saying,” he said. “Even though we may not realize it, the ideas, associations, and content that is reinforced in music shapes the next generation and what society thinks moving forward.”

Bias in lyrics can shape behavior in the same way as language in other forms of communication, the professor said. In business, that could be advertising and marketing materials, or hiring and training materials. For example, if managers are always referred to as male, people unconsciously associate leadership roles with men. Similarly, a 2022 paper found that hiring female CEOs and board members changes organizational language so that the words used to describe women become similar to words that describe agency, such as “independent” and “confident.”

“Culture is a self-reinforcing system,” Berger said. “Both as producers of language and consumers of language, the more we are aware of these biases and try to correct them, the more we can make a more equitable future.”

“The good news is it has gotten better; the bad news is there is still work to do.” — Jonah Berger

Measuring Gender Bias with AI

There have been previous studies on gender bias in music, but they have been limited in scope. Older research looked at a small number of songs, a single genre, or an isolated time period that doesn’t establish long-term trends. And the data had to be manually coded.

Berger has conducted many studies using automated textual analysis, which allows data to be objectively analyzed at scale, so he knew it would be a perfect tool to parse information from a quarter-million songs. But the professors faced another challenge in deciding how to measure bias. Some song lyrics are explicitly violent against women, but most are subtle or open to interpretation.

They decided to focus on whether, compared to men, women are talked about as competent and intelligent or merely warm or kind.

“These subtle linguistic choices have pernicious impacts,” Berger said. “Sure, being nice, warm, and kind are all good things. But when making important decisions, like who to hire or give raise, competence often matters more than warmth.”

When it comes to the pervasiveness of sexism, there is plenty of blame to go around. Berger noted previous research that found protagonists in children’s books tend to be male, while female characters tend to be more passive. He also mentioned a study that found roughly 70% of questions posed to female tennis players weren’t about the sport, like when Serena Williams was asked in 2015 why she wasn’t smiling. Berger writes about similar forms of bias in his most recent book, “ Magic Words: What to Say to Get Your Way .”

“Music isn’t the only place where there is still bias,” he said. “Given how these biases exist in other domains, what we found is not surprising. The good news is it has gotten better; the bad news is there is still work to do.”

More From Knowledge at Wharton

music has no boundaries essay

Isabelle Sakai, Global Chief Marketing Officer of Mark Anthony Brands International

Keith sheldon from seminole gaming and hard rock international.

music has no boundaries essay

Five Myths About Generative AI That Leaders Should Know

Looking for more insights.

Sign up to stay informed about our latest article releases.

Find anything you save across the site in your account

Illustration of a missile made from words.

In the campus protests over the war in Gaza, language and rhetoric are—as they have always been when it comes to Israel and Palestine—weapons of mass destruction.

By Zadie Smith

A philosophy without a politics is common enough. Aesthetes, ethicists, novelists—all may be easily critiqued and found wanting on this basis. But there is also the danger of a politics without a philosophy. A politics unmoored, unprincipled, which holds as its most fundamental commitment its own perpetuation. A Realpolitik that believes itself too subtle—or too pragmatic—to deal with such ethical platitudes as thou shalt not kill. Or: rape is a crime, everywhere and always. But sometimes ethical philosophy reënters the arena, as is happening right now on college campuses all over America. I understand the ethics underpinning the protests to be based on two widely recognized principles:

There is an ethical duty to express solidarity with the weak in any situation that involves oppressive power.

If the machinery of oppressive power is to be trained on the weak, then there is a duty to stop the gears by any means necessary.

The first principle sometimes takes the “weak” to mean “whoever has the least power,” and sometimes “whoever suffers most,” but most often a combination of both. The second principle, meanwhile, may be used to defend revolutionary violence, although this interpretation has just as often been repudiated by pacifistic radicals, among whom two of the most famous are, of course, Mahatma Gandhi and Martin Luther King, Jr . In the pacifist’s interpretation, the body that we must place between the gears is not that of our enemy but our own. In doing this, we may pay the ultimate price with our actual bodies, in the non-metaphorical sense. More usually, the risk is to our livelihoods, our reputations, our futures. Before these most recent campus protests began, we had an example of this kind of action in the climate movement. For several years now, many people have been protesting the economic and political machinery that perpetuates climate change, by blocking roads, throwing paint, interrupting plays, and committing many other arrestable offenses that can appear ridiculous to skeptics (or, at the very least, performative), but which in truth represent a level of personal sacrifice unimaginable to many of us.

I experienced this not long ago while participating in an XR climate rally in London. When it came to the point in the proceedings where I was asked by my fellow-protesters whether I’d be willing to commit an arrestable offense—one that would likely lead to a conviction and thus make travelling to the United States difficult or even impossible—I’m ashamed to say that I declined that offer. Turns out, I could not give up my relationship with New York City for the future of the planet. I’d just about managed to stop buying plastic bottles (except when very thirsty) and was trying to fly less. But never to see New York again? What pitiful ethical creatures we are (I am)! Falling at the first hurdle! Anyone who finds themselves rolling their eyes at any young person willing to put their own future into jeopardy for an ethical principle should ask themselves where the limits of their own commitments lie—also whether they’ve bought a plastic bottle or booked a flight recently. A humbling inquiry.

It is difficult to look at the recent Columbia University protests in particular without being reminded of the campus protests of the nineteen-sixties and seventies, some of which happened on the very same lawns. At that time, a cynical political class was forced to observe the spectacle of its own privileged youth standing in solidarity with the weakest historical actors of the moment, a group that included, but was not restricted to, African Americans and the Vietnamese. By placing such people within their ethical zone of interest, young Americans risked both their own academic and personal futures and—in the infamous case of Kent State—their lives. I imagine that the students at Columbia—and protesters on other campuses—fully intend this echo, and, in their unequivocal demand for both a ceasefire and financial divestment from this terrible war, to a certain extent they have achieved it.

But, when I open newspapers and see students dismissing the idea that some of their fellow-students feel, at this particular moment, unsafe on campus, or arguing that such a feeling is simply not worth attending to, given the magnitude of what is occurring in Gaza, I find such sentiments cynical and unworthy of this movement. For it may well be—within the ethical zone of interest that is a campus, which was not so long ago defined as a safe space, delineated by the boundary of a generation’s ethical ideas— it may well be that a Jewish student walking past the tents, who finds herself referred to as a Zionist, and then is warned to keep her distance, is, in that moment, the weakest participant in the zone. If the concept of safety is foundational to these students’ ethical philosophy (as I take it to be), and, if the protests are committed to reinserting ethical principles into a cynical and corrupt politics, it is not right to divest from these same ethics at the very moment they come into conflict with other imperatives. The point of a foundational ethics is that it is not contingent but foundational. That is precisely its challenge to a corrupt politics.

Practicing our ethics in the real world involves a constant testing of them, a recognition that our zones of ethical interest have no fixed boundaries and may need to widen and shrink moment by moment as the situation demands. (Those brave students who—in supporting the ethical necessity of a ceasefire—find themselves at painful odds with family, friends, faith, or community have already made this calculation.) This flexibility can also have the positive long-term political effect of allowing us to comprehend that, although our duty to the weakest is permanent, the role of “the weakest” is not an existential matter independent of time and space but, rather, a contingent situation, continually subject to change. By contrast, there is a dangerous rigidity to be found in the idea that concern for the dreadful situation of the hostages is somehow in opposition to, or incompatible with, the demand for a ceasefire. Surely a ceasefire—as well as being an ethical necessity—is also in the immediate absolute interest of the hostages, a fact that cannot be erased by tearing their posters off walls.

Part of the significance of a student protest is the ways in which it gives young people the opportunity to insist upon an ethical principle while still being, comparatively speaking, a more rational force than the supposed adults in the room, against whose crazed magical thinking they have been forced to define themselves. The equality of all human life was never a self-evident truth in racially segregated America. There was no way to “win” in Vietnam. Hamas will not be “eliminated.” The more than seven million Jewish human beings who live in the gap between the river and the sea will not simply vanish because you think that they should. All of that is just rhetoric. Words. Cathartic to chant, perhaps, but essentially meaningless. A ceasefire, meanwhile, is both a potential reality and an ethical necessity. The monstrous and brutal mass murder of more than eleven hundred people, the majority of them civilians, dozens of them children, on October 7th, has been followed by the monstrous and brutal mass murder (at the time of writing) of a reported fourteen thousand five hundred children. And many more human beings besides, but it’s impossible not to notice that the sort of people who take at face value phrases like “surgical strikes” and “controlled military operation” sometimes need to look at and/or think about dead children specifically in order to refocus their minds on reality.

To send the police in to arrest young people peacefully insisting upon a ceasefire represents a moral injury to us all. To do it with violence is a scandal. How could they do less than protest, in this moment? They are putting their own bodies into the machine. They deserve our support and praise. As to which postwar political arrangement any of these students may favor, and on what basis they favor it—that is all an argument for the day after a ceasefire. One state, two states, river to the sea—in my view, their views have no real weight in this particular moment, or very little weight next to the significance of their collective action, which (if I understand it correctly) is focussed on stopping the flow of money that is funding bloody murder, and calling for a ceasefire, the political euphemism that we use to mark the end of bloody murder. After a ceasefire, the criminal events of the past seven months should be tried and judged, and the infinitely difficult business of creating just, humane, and habitable political structures in the region must begin anew. Right now: ceasefire. And, as we make this demand, we might remind ourselves that a ceasefire is not, primarily, a political demand. Primarily, it is an ethical one.

But it is in the nature of the political that we cannot even attend to such ethical imperatives unless we first know the political position of whoever is speaking. (“Where do you stand on Israel/Palestine?”) In these constructed narratives, there are always a series of shibboleths, that is, phrases that can’t be said, or, conversely, phrases that must be said. Once these words or phrases have been spoken ( river to the sea, existential threat, right to defend, one state, two states, Zionist, colonialist, imperialist, terrorist ) and one’s positionality established, then and only then will the ethics of the question be attended to (or absolutely ignored). The objection may be raised at this point that I am behaving like a novelist, expressing a philosophy without a politics, or making some rarefied point about language and rhetoric while people commit bloody murder. This would normally be my own view, but, in the case of Israel/Palestine, language and rhetoric are and always have been weapons of mass destruction.

It is in fact perhaps the most acute example in the world of the use of words to justify bloody murder, to flatten and erase unbelievably labyrinthine histories, and to deliver the atavistic pleasure of violent simplicity to the many people who seem to believe that merely by saying something they make it so. It is no doubt a great relief to say the word “Hamas” as if it purely and solely described a terrorist entity. A great relief to say “There is no such thing as the Palestinian people” as they stand in front of you. A great relief to say “Zionist colonialist state” and accept those three words as a full and unimpeachable definition of the state of Israel, not only under the disastrous leadership of Benjamin Netanyahu but at every stage of its long and complex history, and also to hear them as a perfectly sufficient description of every man, woman, and child who has ever lived in Israel or happened to find themselves born within it. It is perhaps because we know these simplifications to be impossible that we insist upon them so passionately. They are shibboleths; they describe a people, by defining them against other people—but the people being described are ourselves. The person who says “We must eliminate Hamas” says this not necessarily because she thinks this is a possible outcome on this earth but because this sentence is the shibboleth that marks her membership in the community that says that. The person who uses the word “Zionist” as if that word were an unchanged and unchangeable monolith, meaning exactly the same thing in 2024 and 1948 as it meant in 1890 or 1901 or 1920—that person does not so much bring definitive clarity to the entangled history of Jews and Palestinians as they successfully and soothingly draw a line to mark their own zone of interest and where it ends. And while we all talk, carefully curating our shibboleths, presenting them to others and waiting for them to reveal themselves as with us or against us—while we do all that, bloody murder.

And now here we are, almost at the end of this little stream of words. We’ve arrived at the point at which I must state clearly “where I stand on the issue,” that is, which particular political settlement should, in my own, personal view, occur on the other side of a ceasefire. This is the point wherein—by my stating of a position—you are at once liberated into the simple pleasure of placing me firmly on one side or the other, putting me over there with those who lisp or those who don’t, with the Ephraimites, or with the people of Gilead. Yes, this is the point at which I stake my rhetorical flag in that fantastical, linguistical, conceptual, unreal place—built with words—where rapes are minimized as needs be, and the definition of genocide quibbled over, where the killing of babies is denied, and the precision of drones glorified, where histories are reconsidered or rewritten or analogized or simply ignored, and “Jew” and “colonialist” are synonymous, and “Palestinian” and “terrorist” are synonymous, and language is your accomplice and alibi in all of it. Language euphemized, instrumentalized, and abused, put to work for your cause and only for your cause, so that it does exactly and only what you want it to do. Let me make it easy for you. Put me wherever you want: misguided socialist, toothless humanist, naïve novelist, useful idiot, apologist, denier, ally, contrarian, collaborator, traitor, inexcusable coward. It is my view that my personal views have no more weight than an ear of corn in this particular essay. The only thing that has any weight in this particular essay is the dead. ♦

New Yorker Favorites

The day the dinosaurs died .

What if you started itching— and couldn’t stop ?

How a notorious gangster was exposed by his own sister .

Woodstock was overrated .

Diana Nyad’s hundred-and-eleven-mile swim .

Photo Booth: Deana Lawson’s hyper-staged portraits of Black love .

Fiction by Roald Dahl: “The Landlady”

Sign up for our daily newsletter to receive the best stories from The New Yorker .

By signing up, you agree to our User Agreement and Privacy Policy & Cookie Statement . This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.

Israel’s Politics of Protest

By Ruth Margalit

The Kids Are Not All Right. They Want to Be Heard

By Keeanga-Yamahtta Taylor

A Generation of Distrust

By Jay Caspian Kang

Is 2024 Doomed to Repeat 1968 or 2020&-or Both?

By Susan B. Glasser

Pebble In The Still Waters

lifestyle, fashion, travel, food, restaurants, destinations, technology, gadgets, health, parenting, family, books, authors, world, India, mobility [email protected]

  • Blog Traffic, Ranks & Stats
  • Social Media Profile
  • Review Policy

Saturday, February 13, 2016

Music has no language barrier, no religion and no boundaries, music has no language barrier. it needs no translation., music has no religion. in fact, it unites people from various religions., music has no boundaries. it has wings to fly. it has the height that nobody can surpass., no comments :, post a comment.

Thanks for visiting and commenting.

badge

IMAGES

  1. Music has no borders, no race or color, no limits of country,

    music has no boundaries essay

  2. Music has no boundaries!

    music has no boundaries essay

  3. Music has no boundaries

    music has no boundaries essay

  4. Top 31 Music Has No Boundaries Quotes: Famous Quotes & Sayings About

    music has no boundaries essay

  5. Music has no boundaries. Elaborate this statement with the help of the

    music has no boundaries essay

  6. Top 31 Music Has No Boundaries Quotes: Famous Quotes & Sayings About

    music has no boundaries essay

COMMENTS

  1. Music may transcend cultural boundaries to become universally human

    Some musical meaning may transcend cultural boundaries and be universally human, study says. Poet and Harvard Professor Henry Wadsworth Longfellow famously said, "Music is the universal language of mankind.". A new Harvard study suggests he may have been right. The study, a collaboration among psychology research associate Samuel Mehr ...

  2. New Harvard study says music is universal language

    November 21, 2019 6 min read. Henry Wadsworth Longfellow wrote, "Music is the universal language of mankind.". Scientists at Harvard have just published the most comprehensive scientific study to date on music as a cultural product, which supports the American poet's pronouncement and examines what features of song tend to be shared ...

  3. Is Music a Universal Language?

    Expressing the shared human experience. Music is a universal language. Or so musicians like to claim. "With music," they'll say, "you can communicate across cultural and linguistic ...

  4. The Power of Music to Help Change the World (and Me!)

    Consequently, even as a 65-year-old musician, I still love making music that pushes boundaries and challenges norms (and hopefully inspires my audience to grow by listening to it), and that has been inspired by a vast array of songs, artists and musical styles from races, cultures, ethnicities and events around the globe.

  5. Music's Universal Pulse: Body and Emotions Unite Across Cultures

    Summary: A groundbreaking cross-cultural study has revealed that music universally influences bodily sensations and emotions, transcending cultural boundaries.Researchers from Western and East Asian backgrounds discovered that emotional and structural characteristics of music consistently evoke similar bodily sensations—such as changes in the chest, limbs, and head regions—regardless of ...

  6. Impact of Music on Society

    According to research, music has a significant impact on humans. It can potentially affect disease, depression, expenditure, productivity, and our outlook on life. The impact of music on our brain is being better understood thanks to advances in neuroscience and the examination of music's impact on the brain.

  7. How Music Can Transcend Language And Impact The World

    Music has been breaking language barriers for quite some time now. An early historical example of music breaking the boundaries of music would be opera. Opera famously began in Italy in the 1500s, and it eventually spread out all across the globe. An art based on storytelling, that somehow managed to wonderfully depict the most epic of tales in ...

  8. Music as a Universal Language: Breaking Barriers and Building

    By Blue Road Music August 15, 2023. Music is often referred to as a universal language that has the remarkable ability to transcend cultural and linguistic barriers. Regardless of the language we speak, the melodies, harmonies, and rhythms of music have the power to evoke emotions and forge connections among people from different backgrounds ...

  9. Study reveals music's universal emotional impact transcends cultural

    Please use one of the following formats to cite this article in your essay, paper or report: APA. Pramanik, Priyanjana. (2024, January 28). Study reveals music's universal emotional impact ...

  10. "Music Has No Borders": An Exploratory Study of ...

    Introduction. COVID-19 is highly contagious and lethal, with a cumulative global infection rate of over 72,000,000 and more than 1,610,000 deaths from December 2019 to the time of writing in December 2020 (Dong et al., 2020).The exponential rates of infection and mortality have necessitated lockdowns in many countries, with wide-ranging social, cultural, economic and political disruptions.

  11. The Universal Language of Nice Music: How Melodies Bridge ...

    Music has an extraordinary ability to transcend boundaries, connecting people from various cultures and backgrounds in profound and meaningful ways. It is a language that speaks to the depths of ...

  12. Music, social cohesion, and intercultural understanding: A conceptual

    Music is a source of cultural understanding and a site for cultural exchange. As a multimodal activity that invites cognitive, emotional, social, and physical engagement, music provides an embodied and situated experience that is deeply intertwined with cultural traditions and attitudes (DeNora, 2000).For these reasons, it has also played a major role in communicating cultural traditions ...

  13. The Relationship Between Music, Culture, and Society: Meaning in Music

    The article considers general issues of determining the competence of cultural self-expression in the context of training future teachers of music through the prism of various foreign scholars and ...

  14. The Relationship Between Music, Culture, and Society ...

    Research about music has generated diverse beliefs, views, and theories that explore its nature and meaning. A recurring theme in the early literature on this topic is the view that the particular cultural context that surrounds a distinct music practice influences the music produced within those cultural boundaries (Herndon & McLeod, 1982; Lomax, 1976; Merriam, 1964; Nettl, 1992).

  15. Charles Rosen's Freedom and the Arts: Essays on Music and Literature

    The critic, historian, music theorist, and virtuosic pianist Charles Rosen has spent the past half century examining this question, and his latest book, Freedom and the Arts: Essays on Music and Literature, a collection of 28 essays written mainly over the past 15 years, is his most expansive analysis to date of the challenges and pleasures of ...

  16. Frontiers

    "There are certain aspects of the so-called 'inner life'—physical or mental —which have formal properties similar to those of music—patterns of motion and rest, of tension and release, of agreement and disagreement, preparation, fulfilment, excitation, sudden change, etc. Langer (1942, p. 228). "The function of music is to enhance in some way the quality of individual experience ...

  17. It's A Small World: Music Can Cross Cultural Barriers And Induce Same

    A recent study, however, may have provided a bit of insight into why the regional song gained international success. The study found that music was able to transcend all cultural and lingual barriers and elicit the same emotions in individuals, regardless of their background.

  18. EMERGING VOICES: Music Has No Boundaries

    EMERGING VOICES: Music Has No Boundaries. This is the eighth and final essay in a series of posts I've written as part of the Emerging Voices project which has been happening at the Philadelphia Chamber Music Society these past two weeks. For more information about the project, ...

  19. Essay on Music for Students and Children

    500+ Words Essay on Music. Music is a vital part of different moments of human life. It spreads happiness and joy in a person's life. Music is the soul of life and gives immense peace to us. In the words of William Shakespeare, "If music is the food of love, play on, Give me excess of it; that surfeiting, The appetite may sicken, and so die

  20. IELTS Essay # 1219

    Write at least 250 words. Model Answer 1: [Agreement] Music plays a crucial role in our lives and eliminates differences among various cultures and generations. It symbolises harmony and reflects humanity. So, I agree with the notion that music helps bring people from various cultures and generations together.

  21. IELTS Writing Task 2: 'music' essay

    IELTS Writing Task 2: 'music' essay. Here's my band 9 sample answer for the question below. Some people say that music is a good way of bringing people of different cultures and ages together. To what extent do you agree or disagree with this opinion? It is often said that music has the power to unite and connect people, regardless of their ...

  22. IELTS Writing Task 2: Music Essay (Band 9)

    1. Sample Band 9. It is often said that music has the power to unite and connect people, regardless of their cultural backgrounds or ages. I completely agree with this view, and will give my reasons below. Music can certainly reach across cultural and national boundaries and bring people together. Perhaps the best example of this would be the ...

  23. Has Music Become Less Misogynistic?

    "Music isn't the only place where there is still bias," he said. "Given how these biases exist in other domains, what we found is not surprising. The good news is it has gotten better; the ...

  24. War in Gaza, Shibboleths on Campus

    Practicing our ethics in the real world involves a constant testing of them, a recognition that our zones of ethical interest have no fixed boundaries and may need to widen and shrink moment by ...

  25. Music Has No Language Barrier, No Religion and No Boundaries

    It heals your heart and soul in a most significant manner. Music has no religion. You look at music as a universal and pure entity. Music has no boundaries. It has wings. It spreads like anything. Music can create momentum, stir, energy and synergies. Music has the power to sync you with the supreme power. It syncs your heart and soul.